Prerequisites for Accepting the Digital Educational Environment in New Cultural and Historical Conditions

249

Abstract

According to cultural historical activity theory motivation, adaptability, study-related experience (“perezhivanie”), and moral codes may be considered as prerequisites for Digital educational environment (DEE) acceptance. To measure the attitudes towards DEE a Scale for Assessing University Digital Edu¬cational Environment was used (AUDEE Scale by M. Sorokova, M. Odintsova, and N. Radchikova). Aca¬demic motivation was evaluated by “Academic Motivation Scales” Questionnaire (by T. Gordeeva, O. Sy¬chev, and E. Osin). Study-related experiences were measured by Activity-Related Experiences Assessment technique (AREA) developed by D. Leontiev and his colleagues. Moral behavior was evaluated with the help of Moral Disengagement Questionnaire (MD-24) adapted by Y. Ledovaya and her colleagues. Stu¬dents’ adaptability was accessed by a questionnaire developed by T. Dubovitskaya and A. Krylova. 406 stu¬dents of Moscow State University of Psychology and Education took part in the investigation (90,1% fe¬male). The average age was 28,7±9,6 years (median = 24 years) varying from 19 to 72 years. The results showed that it is possible to distinguish two groups based on the results of AUDEE scale: Acceptance group and Resistance group. Acceptance group has higher scores in almost all motivation indicators, study-related experiences of pleasure and meaning, adaptability to educational activities, and lower scores in five out of seven moral disengagement strategies. Statistical analysis (classification trees) showed that motiva¬tion (both external and internal) and study-related experience are key resources for the DEE acceptance.

General Information

Keywords: digital educational environment, prerequisites for acceptance / rejection, motivation, cultural-historical conditions, «cultural capability».

Journal rubric: Problems of Cultural-Historical and Activity Psychology

Article type: scientific article

DOI: https://doi.org/10.17759/chp.2021170315

Funding. The reported study was funded by the Moscow State University of Psychology and Education (MSUPE) in the framework of the research project “Digital Technologies in Higher Education: Development of Technology for Individualizing Education Using E-Courses”.

For citation: Radchikova N.P., Odintsova M.A., Sorokova M.G. Prerequisites for Accepting the Digital Educational Environment in New Cultural and Historical Conditions. Kul'turno-istoricheskaya psikhologiya = Cultural-Historical Psychology, 2021. Vol. 17, no. 3, pp. 115–124. DOI: 10.17759/chp.2021170315.

Full text

 

Introduction

The digital revolution has opened up new opportunities for education, quickly and significantly changed it. The digital transformation of education includes the process of using digital tools. In modern cultural and historical conditions, a digital instrument is becoming a familiar cultural instrument. As L. Vygotsky and A. Luria stated: “A cultured person has nothing to strain his sight to see a distant object — he can put on glasses for this, look through binoculars or take a telescope; he does not need to listen to the distance, run as fast as he can to convey the news — he performs all these functions with the help of those instruments and means of communication and transportation that carry out his will. All artificial tools, all cultural environment serve to expand our senses, and a modern cultured person can afford the luxury of possessing them” [35, p. 156]. The luxury of possessing digital tools expands our capabilities, generates new pedagogical practices and forms a new social developmental situation. In such a way, digital technologies arise, on the one hand, as part of the historical heritageof human activity, mediated by tools, and on the other, as powerful multifunctional intermediary means, which significantly expands human capabilities, including in the field of education.

T. Blayone states that modern information technologies, as it were, function “between” people and their environment, increasing opportunities and contributing to both interiorization (construction of individual thinking) and exteriorization (the ability of a person to restructure internalized schemes and reintroduce them into the environment in the form of ideas, artifacts, etc.) [5]. All this occurs against the background of a cultural shift (disruption of the existing balance of the cultural system), in the conditions of a transition from a stable state to an unstable one, and can contribute to both the development of culture, an increase in its creative potential, and stagnation, or even destruction [27]. What is happening in the field of education can be described as a “local cultural revolution” (according to M. Saraf [27]), which inevitably includes a set of profound qualitative changes in education, as a result of which there is a restructuring and transformation of the entire structure of education and its relationships with other areas of life.

The specificity of the new social situation of development requires a revision of views on the process of socialization and the introduction of the concept of “digital socialization” [29]. Digital socialization is understood as “the process of mastering and appropriating a person’s social experience acquired in online contexts, mediated by all available digital technologies, reproducing this experience in mixed offline / online reality and forming his digital personality as part of a real personality” [29, p. 76.]. The problem of digital socialization at a new historical stage in the development of society includes a wide range of issues. These are issues of acceptance / rejection of new technologies; attitudes towards innovation; motivation to study in a digital educational environment (DEE); the quality of experience as characteristic of educational activity; moral codes (academic honesty / dishonesty) as universal components of human culture; the ability to quickly adapt to the changing conditions of digitalization. This is also the problem of satisfaction with online learning; the effectiveness of the use of information and communication technologies, their assessments and factors that determine this effectiveness.

An integrated approach that has become widespread recently [7, 20] proposes to determine the efficiency of online learning as a set of two indicators: the level of knowledge, skills, and abilities and student satisfaction with the experience of online education. Academic performance was often considered in the research, possibly due to its easy availability, while satisfaction with online education and convenience of work in a DEE were rarely examined. That is why students’ attitudes towards the DEE were taken as the indicator of e-learning performance in this work. Students’ attitudes towards DEE, acceptance of DEE are even more important because without acceptance and positive attitude good educational results are impossible.

In such a way, the main question of interest for scientists working in this field concerns the prerequisites and factors of DEE acceptance. According to cultural historical activity theory (CHAT) that includes L. Vy­gotsky’s cultural historical theory and A. Leontiev’s Activity Theory, one of the key prerequisites is motivation. Intrinsic motivation is essential because intention, in fact, is a verbal representation of the future action result. However, the intention represents an order to oneself (“I will do this”) and marks a very important step in correct development of human motivation processes: the emergence, along with external orders, of internal ones, that is, the internalization of speech motivation [24, p. 53]. As the next step in complicating the motivational structure of human actions is mediation our motives by acts of social evaluation, extrinsic motivation is also important. External and internal motivation, correlated with the level of social culture of the individual, contribute to the mutual enrichment of all participants in the educational process. As a result, students from passive recipients of knowledge, skills and abilities become active participants in online learning.

Another important concept for CHAT is “perezhivanie” (experience), which is often not translated into English (e.g., [14]) and has German analogs “das Erleben”, “das Erlebniss”. The concept perezhivanie “allows us to further consider consciousness as a self-generating system that is developed as a new human quality that emerges along with ongoing social life” [14, p. 3]. Perezhivanie appears during human action; it involves emotions and cognition within a complex psychical network that is never defined as a result of a single external cause. The state of experience (perezhivanie) is similar to a state of flow [9] — a state of concentration or complete absorption with the current activity and the situation. It is considered that the flow state is an optimal state of intrinsic motivation, where a person is fully immersed in what they are doing. Sometimes flow experience is characterized as the optimal experience. As optimality could be understood in three different ways [18] as 1) effectiveness, i.e., the achievement of a certain result, 2) positive emotional balance, maximizing positive emotions and minimizing negative, and 3) inclusion in the sense contexts of your own life as well as the lives of other people and society in overall, connection with the past and the future, in the experience of flow all three components are united. Notice that optimal experience includes sense contexts that are close to Vygotsky’s notion of senses (a dynamic system of senses that includes a motivational (affective) side, the will, the dynamics of action and the dynamics of thinking [14]). In such a way, motivation is closely related to experience (perezhivanie) in specific activities [19].

Another factor of acceptance / rejection of online learning is moral codes (academic honesty / dishonesty) as universal components of human culture. According to CHAT the concept of moral identity is culturally biased [16, 25]. Ethics and moral codes remain critical and universal components of human culture and have a strong imprint in language [28]. Language (labeling) plays a great role because mediators (material or symbolic) are considered intrinsic components of higher mental functions. This influence could be found in different mechanisms of moral disengagement [4]. Some studies have shown that most violations of ethical standards occur during online examinations [22]; in a networked environment in general, as many of the characteristics of online technology increase the potential for dishonest behavior. At the same time, students in the online environment are less likely to admit that their actions were wrong [1]. However, some researchers have shown that the Internet and other digital tools are means, but not causes of academic dishonesty [33]. Other factors also contribute to the use of dishonest strategies. For example, under such social norms, where their use is considered acceptable [21]; in collectivist cultures, where there is a greater tolerance for dishonest strategies and moral attitudes towards mutual assistance are widespread [3]. In general, the cultural context plays an important role in shaping students’ attitudes towards academic dishonesty: how acceptable dishonest behavior is and what is meant by morality / immorality [8].

Adaptability allows to reflect the process and results of internal changes, external active adaptation, and self­change of the individual to new conditions of existence and may be important too. Adaptability to learning in a digital educational environment presupposes an optimal combination of internal (subjective) and external (environmental) conditions. On the part of the subject of educational activity, there may be resistance to the introduction of information and communication technologies. Globally, resistance to the introduction of new technologies into culture is in fact resistance to modernity, which prevents successful adaptation to changing environmental conditions [13].

Summarizing all of the above, we note that one of the specific tasks of psychological science is to highlight the prerequisites for the acceptance of digital educational technologies in new cultural and historical conditions. Based on cultural historical activity theory, such prerequisites are motivation to learn in a digital educational environment [24]; experiencing pleasure and meaningfulness of learning activities in new conditions [19]; moral codes (academic honesty) as universal components of human culture [8] and the ability to quickly adapt to educational activities in the context of digitalization [13].

Method

Participants

406 students of Moscow State University of Psychology and Education took part in the investigation (90,1% female). All of them complete online courses on mathematical methods in psychology. The average age was 28,7±9,6 years (median = 24 years, mode = 20 years) varying from 19 to 72 years. The data was obtained in September-December of 2020 when the University worked in distance mode. The database is available at RusPsyDATA [31].

Instruments

To measure the attitudes towards the DEE a special Scale for assessing university digital educational environment was used (AUDEE Scale by M. Sorokova, M. Odintsova, and N. Radchikova, 2021 [32]). AUDEE Scale has six subscales: “DEE Learning Process Satisfaction”, “DEE Communication Satisfaction and Learning Motivation”, “DEE Stress Tension”, “Need for Support in the DEE Learning Activities”, “DEE Dishonest Strategies Prevalence”, and “DEE Accessibility” as well as the total score indicating the degree of positive attitude.

Academic motivation was evaluated by “Academic Motivation Scales” Questionnaire (by T. Gordeeva, O. Sychev, and E. Osin, 2014 [15]) based on self-determination theory describing the intrinsic and extrinsic motivation of academic activity. It is assumed that different types of motivation related to needs are satisfied by the nature of the learning activity (such as needs for cognition, achievement, and personal growth) and needs external to learning (such as needs for autonomy and self-respect). The Questionnaire has seven scales, including three scales measuring types of intrinsic motivation (intrinsic cognition, achievement, and personal growth), three scales measuring extrinsic motivation (motivation for self-respect, introjected, and external regulation), and an amotivation scale.

Study-related experiences were measured by Activity-Related Experiences Assessment technique (AREA) developed by D. Leontiev and his colleagues in 2018 [18]. This technique allows to determine four different experiences one could have when studying: Effort, Pleasure, Meaning, and Void. The experience of effort tells us about the effectiveness of the activity, the experience of pleasure — about its pleasantness, and the experience of meaning — about its involvement in more broad contexts. The absence of all three components is manifested in the experience of emptiness (void), which could be described as psychic entropy, a sensation being a victim of uncontrolled processes [18, p. 57].

Moral behavior was evaluated with the help of Moral Disengagement Questionnaire (MD-24) based on A. Bandura’s concept of moral disengagement and adapted by Y. Ledovaya and her colleagues in 2016 [17]. According to A. Bandura’s concept of moral, disengagement has eight different mechanisms: Moral Justification, Euphemistic Labelling, Advantageous Comparison, Displacement of Responsibility, Diffusion of Responsibility, Distortion of Consequences, Dehumanization, Attribution of Blame.

Students’ adaptability was accessed by a special questionnaire that includes two scales: adaptability to the study group and adaptability to the educational activity developed in 2010 by T. Dubovitskaya and A. Krylova [12].

Statistical packages SPSS V.23 and SAS JMP 11.0.0 were used for statistical analysis.

Results and discussion

Descriptive statistics for all measured variables are presented in Table 1. Average values of Moral Disengagement Scales lie in the range between 1,5 and 3,1 that corresponds with the data obtained by Y. Ledovaya et al. [17]. The scores for adaptability to the study group and adaptability to educational activities are also similar to
those obtained by T. Dubovitskaya and A. Krylova
(12,0 and 10,6 respectively) [12]. The results of motivation measurement are higher for some scales and lower for some scales than the results of T. Gordeeva et al. [15]. For example, achievement motivation is two points more for our sample, introjected motivation is two points less, but the difference between the means lies within two points.

Cluster analysis (k-means) was used to divide all the participants into groups on the basis of their attitude towards DEE (AUDEE Scale). All AUDEE subscales were used. All of them were standardized because of different ranges. The results show that two different attitudes towards DEE (two clusters) could be distinguished (pict. 1). One group of participants (cluster 2, N = 221) is characterized by the satisfaction with the learning process and communication above average, by learning motivation above average, by high evaluation of DEE accessibility, by stress tension below average, by need for support below average, and by low estimation of dishonest strategies prevalence. It could be assumed that the members of this group accept DEE technology, and therefore it may be called an Acceptance group. The other group of participants (cluster 1, N = 185) is characterized by the satisfaction in the learning process and communication below average, by learning motivation below average, by low evaluation of DEE accessibility, by stress tension above average, by need for support above average, and by high estimation of dishonest strategies prevalence. All this indicates that the members of the group try to resist DEE technology and avoid it. Further, we will refer to it as Resistance group.

To determine what characteristics are specific for each group Student’s t-test was applied. The comparison of the two groups (table 2) shows that they differ almost in all study-related experiences: pleasure experience and meaning experience are significantly higher for Acceptance group, Void experience is significantly lower.

 

Table 1

Descriptive statistics for all measured variables: mean ± standard deviation, median [lower quartile; upper quartile], minimum, and maximum (N=406)

Questionnaires and scales

M ± SD

Me

[LQ; UQ]

Min

Max

Activity-Related Experiences Assessment technique (AREA)

Effort

12,8 ± 3,4

13 [11; 15]

3

18

Pleasure

11,5 ± 3,9

12 [9; 14]

3

18

Meaning

14,0 ± 3,7

15 [12; 17]

3

18

Void

7,7 ± 3,6

7 [5; 10]

3

18

“Academic Motivation Scales” Questionnaire

Intrinsic cognition

16,6 ± 3,0

17 [15; 19]

4

20

Achievement

15,9 ± 3,1

16 [14; 18]

4

20

Personal growth

16,1 ± 3,1

16 [14; 19]

4

20

Self-respect

13,6 ± 4,1

14 [11; 16]

4

20

Introjected motivation

11,2 ± 3,9

11 [8; 14]

4

20

External regulation

9,2 ± 3,7

9 [6; 12]

4

20

Amotivation

7,1 ± 3,4

6 [4; 9]

4

20

Students Adaptability in the Higher Educational Establishment

Adaptability to Study Group

12,0 ± 3,3

13 [10; 15]

0

16

Adaptability to Educational Activities

10,9 ± 3,8

12 [8; 14]

0

16

Moral Disengagement Scale (MD-24)

Moral Justification

2,2 ± 1,3

2 [1; 3]

1

7

Euphemistic Labelling

1,5 ± 0,9

1 [1; 2]

1

7

Advantageous Comparison

2,9 ± 1,4

3 [2; 4]

1

7

Displacement of Responsibility

2,4 ± 1,3

2 [1; 3]

1

6

Diffusion of Responsibility

2,4 ± 1,3

2 [1; 3]

1

7

Distortion of Consequences

2,0 ± 1,3

2 [1; 3]

1

7

Dehumanization

2,4 ± 1,5

2 [1; 3]

1

7

Attribution of Blame

3,1 ± 1,6

3 [2; 4]

1

7

AUDEE Scale

DEE Learning Process Satisfaction

45,7 ± 8,5

46,0 [40; 52]

13

60

DEE Communication satisfaction and Learning Motivation

19,4 ± 6,2

19,5 [15; 24]

7

35

DEE Stress Tension

20,5 ± 6,6

20,0 [15; 25]

8

40

Need for support in DEE learning activity

14,2 ± 4,6

14,0 [11; 17]

6

28

DEE Dishonest Strategies Prevalence

16,7 ± 3,8

16,0 [14; 19]

7

30

DEE Accessibility

21,4 ± 3,0

22,0 [20; 24]

11

25

AUDEE Scale total score

133,1 ± 23,4

134 [116; 150]

54

184

 

 

However, there is no difference in effort. That may be interpreted in a way that both groups highly appreciated the effectiveness of their activities in the digital educational environment due to perseverance.

Almost all kinds of motivation were higher in Acceptance group compared to Resistance group, and amo- tivation was respectively lower. The only exception is introjected motivation. This means that the Acceptance group is characterized by more pronounced aspirations: to learn new things, to achieve high results in studies, to develop their abilities, in contrast to the Resistance group. At the same time, for those and others, educational activity is based on a sense of duty to significant people.

It is interesting that the adaptability to the study group does not differ, but the adaptability to learning process is one point higher for Acceptance group.

The comparisons of different moral disengagement mechanisms show that although the differences in mean values are not big (from 0,2 to 0,4 scale points), they are statistically significant for euphemistic labelling, displacement and diffusion of responsibility, distortion of consequences, and dehumanization. All mechanisms of moral separation are more pronounced in the Resistance group than in the Acceptance group. Moral responsibility that discourages academic dishonesty reduces its use, but can be neutralized through various mechanisms.

Thus, the Resistance group believes that in fact the use of dishonest strategies is not a deception (Euphemistic Labelling); and the appropriation of other people’s ideas is not such a serious offense (Distortion of Consequences); that dishonest strategies are provoked by educators or fellow students (Displacement of Responsibility); and that everyone uses dishonest strategies (Diffusion of Responsibility); some people can be dispensed with without ceremony (Dehumanization).

It may be supposed that the Resistance group appreciates the mechanisms of moral disconnection more highly due to the belief that the Internet increases the tendency to neutralize moral responsibility (“the online disinhibition effect” [33, 34]). The mechanism of dehumanization emerges due to difficulties in identifying a person, a certain impersonality of the image of a person in the digital environment. However, these assumptions require additional research.

O. Dremova and her colleagues showed that the use of dishonest strategies may be due to negative experiences in learning activities (fear, boredom, dislike for learning activities, etc.) [10, 11]. Positive experiences in learning activities (interest, inspiration, satisfaction with the learning process, etc.) prevent the use of dishonest strategies. This is confirmed by the data obtained in our study for the Resistance group: reduced satisfaction with the educational process and communicative interaction, pronounced stress intensity, the experience of emptiness and meaninglessness of educational activity.

 Table 2

Descriptive statistics (mean ± standard deviation) and the results of comparison of two groups (Student’s test t-value and probability level p)

Variable

Mean ± Std.Dev.

t

P

Acceptance group (N=221)

Resistance group (N=185)

Age, years

28,4 ± 9,2

29,0 ± 10,1

-0,6

0,5687

Activity-Related Experiences Assessment technique (AREA)

Effort

12,8 ± 3,5

12,8 ± 3,4

-0,1

0,8844

Pleasure

13,5 ± 3,0

9,1 ± 3,4

13,7

0,0001

Meaning

15,3 ± 2,7

12,4 ± 4,1

8,7

0,0001

Void

6,3 ± 2,8

9,4 ± 3,6

-9,9

0,0001

“Academic Motivation Scales” Questionnaire

Intrinsic cognition

17,4 ± 2,6

15,7 ± 3,1

5,9

0,0001

Achievement

16,5 ± 3,1

15,1 ± 3,0

4,5

0,0001

Personal growth

16,8 ± 3,0

15,2 ± 2,9

5,6

0,0001

Self-respect

14,2 ± 4,3

13,0 ± 3,8

2,8

0,0060

Introjected motivation

11,2 ± 4,1

11,2 ± 3,7

-0,1

0,9169

External regulation

8,9 ± 3,9

9,7 ± 3,5

-2,1

0,0328

Amotivation

6,2 ± 2,8

8,1 ± 3,8

-5,9

0,0001

Students Adaptability in the Higher Educational Establishment

Adaptability to Study Group

12,2 ± 3,2

11,7 ± 3,3

1,6

0,1149

Adaptability to Educational Activities

11,3 ± 3,7

10,3 ± 3,8

2,6

0,0091

Moral Disengagement Scale (MD-24)

Moral Justification

2,2 ± 1,4

2,2 ± 1,3

-0,3

0,7792

Euphemistic Labelling

1,4 ± 0,8

1,6 ± 1,0

-2,9

0,0044

Advantageous Comparison

2,9 ± 1,5

3,0 ± 1,3

-0,8

0,4216

Displacement of Responsibility

2,2 ± 1,2

2,6 ± 1,3

-2,7

0,0069

Diffusion of Responsibility

2,2 ± 1,2

2,6 ± 1,3

-3,6

0,0004

Distortion of Consequences

1,9 ± 1,2

2,3 ± 1,4

-3,2

0,0013

Dehumanization

2,2 ± 1,4

2,6 ± 1,5

-3,1

0,0022

Attribution of Blame

3,0 ± 1,7

3,2 ± 1,5

-1,3

0,1991

Thus, the Acceptance group is characterized by satisfaction with the educational process and communicative interaction in the DEE, high ratings of the DEE availability, a sense of security, and independence (no need for support in the DEE). Specific features for this group are the following: experiences of pleasure and meaningfulness of educational activity in the DEE; increased motivation to learn new things, to achieve high results in studies, to develop their abilities; sufficient adaptation to the educational process), and lower ratings for the use of dishonest strategies. The Resistance group is characterized by insufficient satisfaction with the educational process and communicative interaction in the DEE, low assessments of accessibility, stress, and a pronounced need for support. This group differs by the experience of emptiness and meaninglessness in educational activity, reduced motivation, insufficient adaptation to the educational process, but sufficient adaptation to the educational group; by usage of some mechanisms of moral separation to neutralize moral responsibility. However, this group is characterized by tenacity and perseverance when working in DEE.

To determine what are the most important characteristics that distinguish Acceptance group a classification tree was built. Classification (or decision) tree analysis is a statistical method which repeatedly splits up a sample into subgroups [6, 26]. The resulted tree for Acceptance group is presented in pict. 2, including the classification variable and the cut point for each split. Within each node the proportion of participants who have got to the Acceptance group between the baseline and follow-up assessments is shown. The model appeared to be very good (AUROC = 0,82; sensitivity = 77%, specificity = 76%, positive predictive value = 79%).

The model (pict. 2) shows that the main predictors to get into the Acceptance group are motivation (Achievement motivation and Self-Respect motivation) and study-related experiences (Pleasure and Void). Achievement motivation of more than 16 points (median value, table 1) increases the chance to get into the Acceptance group from 41,2 % to 63,5 %. For highly motivated students (Achievement motivation > 16 points), pleasure experience is the most important construct. If the pleasure is equal or more than 12 points (median value, table 1), then the chance to get into the Acceptance group increases from 31,3 % to 79,5 %. Very high Self Respect motivation that represents extrinsic motivation (more than 16 points, i.e., more than the upper quartile, table 1) increases chances to get into the Accep­
tance group for more than 10 % (from 74,3 % to 88,3 %). The minimal chance to get into the Acceptance group (3,6 %) is predicted for students with Achievement motivation below average (less than 16 points), pleasure study-related experience lower than 9 points (less than low quartile), and void study-related experience equal to or more than 11 points which is higher than upper quar­tile. This chance is a little higher (23,1 %) if Void study- related experience is less than 11 points.

The risk group with predicted chances to get to the Acceptance group around 8 % is characterized by sufficiently high intrinsic motivation (Achievement motivation > 16 points) but low pleasure study related experience (less than 12 points) and high void study experience (more than 10 points). All this shows that motivation and activity-related experience are important prerequisites for new educational technology acceptance. Adaptability and moral behavior are less important for the prediction of DEE acceptance / rejection. Firstly, this is due to the already accomplished shift in the culture of society, where information technologies have been widely introduced for several decades. The participants of our study, having passed through the “threshold” state between two stages of cultural development (pre-informa-tional and informational), adapted to these conditions in their daily activities. They have space for individual transformation in the educational environment. After all, “an adult is not only connected with the environment by thousands of intimate connections — he himself is its product, his essence is in the essence of his environment” [35, p. 130]. “The environment, as it were, grow inward, behavior becomes social, cultural, not only in its content, but also in its mechanisms, in its techniques” [35, p. 157]. Such “growing inward” involves the process of adaptation to new conditions. At the present stage, there is an increase in the overall level of digital competencies, a culture of digital citizenship is being formed [29] regardless of age [30]. Second, the use of dishonest strategies in education is global and widespread throughout the world. increasingly, students see certain acts of academic dishonesty as appropriate, common and acceptable in education, regardless of whether they accept or not accept the digital learning environment [2]. In addition, it is possible that collectivism [23], characteristic of Russian culture, is a sufficiently significant factor predicting joint forms of academic dishonesty.

Conclusion

As we can see, the predictions of cultural historical activity theory that postulates that motivation, experiencing pleasure and meaningfulness of learning activities in new conditions, moral codes (academic honesty), and the ability to quickly adapt to educational activities in the context of digitalization could be seen as prerequisites for the DEE acceptance were empirically confirmed. The results of our research show that the prerequisites for the DEE acceptance are the following: experience of pleasure and meaningfulness of educational activity; increased motivation to learn new things, to achieve high results in studies, to develop their abilities; sufficient adaptability to the educational process. Prerequisites for DEE resistance are the following: the experience of emptiness and meaninglessness of educational activity; decreased motivation; insufficient adaptability to the educational process; using some mechanisms of moral separation to neutralize moral responsibility.

The unifying characteristics of the two groups distinguished by the attitude towards the DEE (Acceptance group and Resistance group) are persistence and perseverance when working in the DEE and successful adaptation to the study group. These characteristics can be viewed as resources for embracing digital reality in education, reducing tensions and determining the success of information technology adoption.

Thus, according to the apt remark of L. Vygotsky and A. Luria, “Culture, the environment remakes a person, not only giving him certain knowledge. They transform the very structure of his psychological processes, developing in him certain methods of using his own capabilities” [35, p. 221]. We see that every person has these opportunities, but not everyone can use them for their intended purpose due to various reasons and barriers.

 

To some extent, our research allowed us to only partially understand these reasons. In the future, it is planned to study the “cultural capability” of modern man as a dynamic phenomenon, acquired in live contact with the social environment, highlighted by L. Vygotsky and A. Luria. This phenomenon is of particular relevance today as a result of the complication, enrichment of the cultural environment and the growing demands on the resources and capabilities of the person himself.

 

 

 

 

 

References

  1. Akimov A., Malin M. When old becomes new: a case study of oral examination as an online assessment tool. Assessment & Evaluation in Higher Education, 2020. Vol. 45, no. 8, pp. 1205— 1221 (In Russ.). DOI:10.1080/02602938.2020.1730301
  2. Albers N.D., Knotts T.L. Cultural Influences on Academic Sharing: A Challenge to Academic Honesty. In J. Keengwe (ed.) Handbook of Research on Cross-Cultural Online Learning in Higher Education, 2019, pp. 230—252. DOI:10.4018/978-1-5225-8286-1.ch012
  3. Aljurf S., Kemp L. J., Williams P. Exploring academic dishonesty in the Middle East: a qualitative analysis of students’ perceptions. Studies in Higher Education, 2020. Vol. 45, no. 7, pp. 1461—1473. DOI:10.1080/03075079.2018.1564262
  4. Bandura A. Moral Disengagement in the Perpetration of Inhumanities. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 1999, no. 3(3), pp. 193—209. DOI:10.1207/s15327957pspr0303_3.
  5. Blayone T.J.B. Theorizing effective uses of digital technology with activity theory. Technology, Pedagogy and Education, 2019. Vol. 28, no. 4, pp. 447—462. DOI:10.1080/1 475939X.2019.1645728
  6. Breiman L. et al. Classification and regression trees. Brooks/Cole Publishing, Monterey, 1984. 358 p.
  7. Castro M.D.B., Tumibay G.M. A literature review: efficacy of online learning courses for higher education institution using meta-analysis. Education and Information Technologies, 2019. Vol. 24, pp. 1—19. DOI:10.1007/s10639-019-10027-z
  8. Chudzicka-Czupała A. et al. Application of the Theory of Planned Behavior in Academic Cheating Research—Cross- Cultural Comparison. Ethics & Behavior, 2016. Vol. 26, no. 8, pp. 638—659. DOI: 10.1080/10508422.2015.1112745]
  9. Csikszentmihalyi M. Flow: The Psychology of Optimal Experience. New York: Harper and Row, 1990. 336 p.
  10. Dremova O.V. Student academic dishonesty: Review of theoretical frameworks and methods of prevention. Pedagogy and Psychology of Education, 2020, no. 2, pp. 93—111 (In Russ.) DOI:10.31862/2500-297X-2020-2-93-111
  11. Dremova O.V., Maloshonok N.G., Terentiev E.A. Seeking Justice in Academia: Criticism and Justification of Student Academic Dishonesty. Monitoring of Public Opinion: Economic and Social Changes, 2020, no. 4, pp. 366—394 (In Russ.). DOI:10.14515/monitoring.2020.4.972.
  12. Dubovitskaya T.D., Krylova A.V. Method of Research of Students Adaptability in the Higher Educational Establishment. Psikhologicheskaya nauka i obrazovanie = Psychological Science and Education, 2010, no. 2 (In Russ.) DOI:10.17759/psyedu.
  13. France D. et al. Should you be using mobile technologies in teaching? Applying a pedagogical framework. Journal of Geography in Higher Education, 2020. Vol. 45, no. 2, pp. 221— 237 DOI:10.1080/03098265.2020.1773417.
  14. Gonzalez Rey F. Human Motivation in Question: Discussing Emotions, Motives, and Subjectivity from a Cultural-Historical Standpoint. Journal for the Theory of Social Behaviour, 2014, no. 45(3), pp. 1—18. DOI:10.1111/jtsb.12073.
  15. Gordeeva T., Sychev O., Osin E. “Academic motivation scales” questionnaire. Psikhologicheskii Zhurnal, 2014, no. 35, pp. 96—107 (In Russ.).
  16. Jia F., Krettenauer T. Recognizing Moral Identity as a Cultural Construct. Frontiers in psychology, 2017, no. 8, p. 412. DOI:10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00412.
  17. Ledovaya Y. et. al. Moral disengagement: the psychological construct and its measurement. Vestnik of St Petersburg University. Series 16. Psychology and Education, 2016, no. 6, pp. 23—39 (In Russ.). DOI:10.21638/11701/spbu16.2016.
  18. Leontiev D.A. et al. Study-Related Experiences and Their Association with Psychological Well-Being. Psychological Science and Education, 2018. Vol. 23, no. 6, pp. 55—66 (In Russ.). DOI:10.17759/pse.2018230605.
  19. Leontiev D.A., Klein K.G. The quality of motivation and the quality of experiences as characteristics of learning activity. Moscow university psychology bulletin, 2018, no 4, p. 106—119 (In Russ.). DOI:10.11621/vsp.2018.04.106
  20. Leybina A.V., Shukuryan G.A. Ways to enhance the effectiveness of online education. Sovremennaya zarubezhnaya psikhologiya = Journal of Modern Foreign Psychology, 2020. Vol. 9, no. 3, pp. 21—33 (In Russ.). DOI:10.17759/ jmfp.2020090302.
  21. Malesky A. et al. The Effects of Peer Influence, Honor Codes, and Personality Traits on Cheating Behavior in a University Setting. Ethics & Behavior, 2021, 1—11. DOI:10.10 80/10508422.2020.1869006
  22. Olafson L. et al. Exploring the Judgment—Action Gap: College Students and Academic Dishonesty. Ethics & Behavior, 2013. Vol. 23, no. 2, pp. 148—162. DOI:10.1080/105 08422.2012.714247
  23. Orosz G. et al. Linking cheating in school and corruption. European Review of Applied Psychology, 2018. Vol. 68, no. 2, pp. 89—97. DOI:10.1016/j.erap.2018.02.001.
  24. Patyaeva E.Yu. Towards a Cultural-Historical Theory of Motivation: Pierre Janet’s Conception of Levels in Human Actions. Kul’turno-istoricheskaya psikhologiya = Cultural- Historical Psychology, 2011. Vol. 7, no. 1, pp. 49—62 (In Russ.).
  25. Purzycki B.G. et al. The cognitive and cultural foundations of moral behavior. Evolution and Human Behavior, 2018. Vol. 39, Issue 5, pp. 490—501, DOI:10.1016/j. evolhumbehav.2018.04.004.
  26. Quinlan J.R. Induction of Decision Trees. Machine Learning, 1986, no. 1, pp. 81—106.
  27. Saraf M.Ya. Measuring of the cultural space. Prostranstvo i vremya — Space and Time, 2013, no. 1. pp. 58—68 (In Rus).
  28. Saucier G. Culture, morality and individual differences: comparability and incomparability across species. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society b, 2018. Vol. b373: 20170170. http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2017.0170
  29. Soldatova G.U. Digital socialization in the cultural-historical paradigm: a changing child in a changing world. Social psychology and society, 2018. Vol. 9, no. 3, рр. 71—80 (In Rus). DOI:10.17759/sps.2018090308.
  30. Sorokova M.G. Skepticism and learning difficulties in a digital environment at the Bachelor’s and Master’s levels: are preconceptions valid? Heliyon, 2020. Vol. 6, no. 11, E05335 (In Rus). DOI:10.1016/j.heliyon.2020.e05335.
  31. Sorokova M.G., Odintsova M.A., Radchikova N.P. AUDEE Scale Data. RusPsyData, 2021. IN PRESS Replaced: Sorokova M.G., Odintsova M.A., Radchikova N.P. AUDEE Scale: validation database. Psychological Research Data & Tools Repository. Dataset, 2021. DOI:10.25449/ ruspsydata.14742816
  32. Sorokova M.G., Odintsova M.A., Radchikova N. Scale for Assessing University Digital Educational Environment (AUDEE Scale). Psikhologicheskaya nauka i obrazovanie = Psychological Science and Education, 2021. Vol. 26, no. 2, pp. 52—65. DOI:10.17759/pse.2021260205. (In Russ.)
  33. Stephens J.M., Young M.F., Calabrese T. Does Moral Judgment Go Offline When Students Are Online? A Comparative Analysis of Undergraduates’ Beliefs and Behaviors Related to Conventional and Digital Cheating. Ethics & Behavior, 2007. Vol. 17, no. 3, pp. 233—254. DOI:10.1080/10508420701519197
  34. Suler J. The Online Disinhibition Effect. CyberPsychology & Behavior, 2004, no. 7 (3), pp. 321—326. DOI:10.1089/1094931041291295
  35. Vygotskii L.S. Luriya A.R. Etyudy po istorii povedeniya: obez’yana. Primitiv. Rebenok. [Studies in the history of behavior: monkey. Primitive. Child]. Moscow: Pedagogika-Press, 1993. 224 p. (In Rus).

Information About the Authors

Nataly P. Radchikova, PhD in Psychology, Leading Researcher of Scientific and Practical Center for Comprehensive Support of Psychological Research «PsyDATA», Moscow State University of Psychology & Education, Chief Specialist of the Laboratory of Biophysics of Excitable Media, Institute of Theoretical and Experimental Biophysics, Russian Academy of Sciences, Pushchino;, Moscow, Russia, ORCID: https://orcid.org/0000-0002-5139-8288, e-mail: nataly.radchikova@gmail.com

Maria A. Odintsova, PhD in Psychology, Docent, Head of the Department of Psychology and Pedagogy of Distance Learning, Faculty of Distance Learning, Moscow State University of Psychology & Education, Moscow, Russia, ORCID: https://orcid.org/0000-0003-3106-4616, e-mail: mari505@mail.ru

Marina G. Sorokova, Doctor of Education, PhD in Physics and Matematics, docent, Head of Scientific and Practical Center for Comprehensive Support of Psychological Research "PsyDATA", Head of the Department of Digital Education, Moscow State University of Psychology and Education, Moscow, Russia, ORCID: https://orcid.org/0000-0002-1000-6487, e-mail: sorokovamg@mgppu.ru

Metrics

Views

Total: 666
Previous month: 8
Current month: 13

Downloads

Total: 249
Previous month: 2
Current month: 5