Psychological Science and Education
2022. Vol. 27, no. 3, 16–27
doi:10.17759/pse.2022270302
ISSN: 1814-2052 / 2311-7273 (online)
Learning Space as a Prerequisite of Agency in Learning Activity
Abstract
The rapid pace of innovation and the increased quantity of information are affecting the traditional educational routes. Schools are now facing quite a new task: how to teach children to learn. The developmental learning approach of Elkonin and Davidov provides rich experience of solving this task. The paper describes a technology of learning space polarization that promotes learning autonomy in primary school and has been successfully applied in developmental learning classes. We explore the prerequisites of individual learning action formation, the action which is self-motivated, independent and responsible. We also describe three lines of learning autonomy development in students: result, research and product. The paper concludes with a description of the evolution of learning autonomy and its social/institutional forms and relates its stages to certain age periods in the child development.
General Information
Keywords: learning autonomy, learning space, individual learning action, developmental learning, training and inquiry-based lessons, modeling, sign tools, Elkonin, Davydov, agency, activity
Journal rubric: Educational Psychology
Article type: scientific article
DOI: https://doi.org/10.17759/pse.2022270302
Funding. The reported study was funded by National Research University Higher School of Economics, within the project “Development of preschool and primary school children’s autonomy” (Mirror laboratory)
Received: 28.02.2022
Accepted:
For citation: Ostroverkh O.S. Learning Space as a Prerequisite of Agency in Learning Activity. Psikhologicheskaya nauka i obrazovanie = Psychological Science and Education, 2022. Vol. 27, no. 3, pp. 16–27. DOI: 10.17759/pse.2022270302.
Full text
Introduction
The fundamental result of developmental learning in the Elkonin–Davydov system is the student’s agency. In general terms, an agency is a way of life in which an individual constructs (generates) forms of their behaviour, in the case of schools, their learning experience [2,10,15,16]. Education in all stages is seen as a progression of agency [1, 11, 12, 13, 17, 19, 25, 28].
Education aims to create the conditions for the emergence and development of learning skills. The interpsychic form of learning independence in schoolchildren has been studied most thoroughly in G. A. Tsukerman's research. The core of this form is the exploratory activity that occurs in cooperative learning activities and is focused on discovering and trying out new ways of action [11].
Analysing the work of foreign researchers, we can note that the concept of a personal initiative developed by D. Fay and M. Frese describes the personal initiative as autonomous and proactive behaviour that aims to overcome obstacles to achieve goals. There are three aspects of personal initiative: self-starting, proactivity and perseverance. Self-starting means that a person does something without direct instructions and the initiative aims to change oneself or the situation. Someone who shows personal initiative takes responsibility for an idea or project [20, 21, 23].
We believe that decision-making initiative as a vital aspect of independent action can occur at a very young age. The adult can participate in joint action with the child so that the child has the opportunity and necessity to make choices within the boundaries available at each age. [8, 9, 19, 22, 24, 25, 26, 27].
Currently, the issue of individual aspects of learning skills learn is not sufficiently elaborated in the theory and practice of developmental learning. This article is about how a specific educational space for learning activity created by the teacher promotes the establishment and development of learning autonomy in primary students. We will also explore: what are the symptoms and dynamics of learning autonomy in primary schoolchildren; what is the teacher's agency; what resources and means do teachers have for cultivating students' learning autonomy.
Individual Learning Action in Primary School Students
Learning autonomy relates to the acceptance or rejection of ‘alien’ tasks, to the emergence of one’s own learning goals and implies that the student chooses or constructs the ways to achieve these goals. Independence is marked by the initiative to turn to another person. If any difficulties arise, the student can turn for help to the teacher, a friend, a book, etc. In the learning process, the child can and should decide for themselves whether it is necessary to improve their learning skills. In other words, learning autonomy involves the independent setting of individual learning goals, proactive search for ways to achieve them and responsible decision-making in a situation of choice [5, 7].
We consider the individual learning action as the first stage in the formation of learning autonomy as a personal development that first occurs in primary school age.
The individual learning action implies that the action is proactive and responsible. The initiative action is an action of a child when they transform the adult’s task into their own rather than simply accept it. In contrast to the initiative in general, the learning initiative is associated with the reformulation of the task.
The responsible action involves a certain amount of risk and decision-making. For a child to be able to make decisions meaningfully, they must understand when they are ready to do something and when they are not. The choice, in this sense, is a choice each time between ‘acting in a socially constructed way’ or ‘not acting.’ If the teacher creates situations of choice for the child, then there’s a space for children's initiative in making a decision.
The learning action is an action which is not about improving a person, their abilities but rather about improving the very way of doing some activity.
Earlier in our works, we have shown that the main way of constructing the individual learning action as an action of initiative, independent and responsible decision-making involves dividing the children's actions into preparatory and executive. If the teacher helps children to distinguish and switch between orientation and implementation from the very start, then the individual learning action emerges and develops as an action of initiative, autonomy and responsibility [1, 6].
Learning Space as Factor for Development of Learning Autonomy
To put children's search, trial and preparation at the centre of the teacher's attention, the learning space needs to be organised specially.
S. Zaitsev in his research also indicates the need to create a varied educational environment that should stimulate students to perform learning activities independently and provide a choice of means and ways of accomplishing them [3].
In our case, the main method of teaching is the polarisation of the learning space towards the preparation and implementation:
- the introduction of the draft and its construction as a particular place for preparation;
- special organisation of the subject/space environment’
- the introduction of lesson, class and polarised lesson as different forms for the organisation of learning time and learning activities.
When a teacher focuses on the development of learning autonomy in primary students, the object and subject of the teacher's work change. The object of teaching action is the structure of children's action in terms of the two functions existing in one action and often merged: preparation (orientation) and implementation (execution). The teacher arranges the learning space in a way that enables the student to prepare for any action in a given class, in other words, to construct a way of solving a group of tasks. From the beginning, this task space should act as a preparation space where the child builds his or her own experience of the activity.
The central position in the developmental learning system is the nature of a scientific concept. As V. Davydov pointed out, mastering a concept means knowing how to construct this concept, and tools and signs form the main content of the orientational basis of action [2].
The teacher's action is more related to the initiation of the students' orienting and, more broadly, preparatory actions. Learning to make drafts does not involve handing over ready-made tools, but is linked to the organisation of students' reflective attitude towards their work - their understanding of the relevance of their preparation.
To understand means to learn, according to D. B. Elkonin, it does not necessarily mean to understand but to train and improve yourself in what you are not good at, to work with yourself on some task [16].
Rethinking becomes the focus of communication between the child and adult in the work with the draft. First, the teacher starts to see the child's work as a ‘draft’, giving significance to the child's action as a trial, and then the child rethinks their work in this way [5,6,7]. For example, a first-grader takes a dictation in Russian at the end of first grade. The student writes the whole dictation, then checks the work, finds the misspelt word and circles it. The observer: ‘Why did you highlight that word?’ Student: ‘It's a dictation. I'll practice with the wrong words at home.’ In this example, you can see how the girl plans her work in advance. Self-work is seen both as performance (writing dictation) and as preparation for future action (highlighting words with mistakes).
In our view, when a child gives meaning to their work as the one which can be continued, they build coherence of preparation and implementation, i.e. an individual learning action.
The distinctions and transitions between the two functional parts of an action—between orientation, preparation in the broad sense and realisation—become the subject of the teacher's observation and work. The teacher considers not only how the child has mastered the content of the lesson but also how they organise the preparation, whether they are proactive in using the resources, how they act in a situation of difficulty and at what point they decide to end the preparation. Teachers' action becomes transparent when it turns into children's action, which is independent rather than imitating an adult's model [14].
Here we are talking about constructing teaching activities that reveal to the child the meaning of their action and provide an understanding of how to transform their way of doing things. This transformation has a proactive and responsible form of behaviour. Initiative, responsibility and learning are generated as an educational outcome and cannot be shaped directly.
The individual features in the forms of training can already be observed by the middle of the first grade, which is one of the symptoms of the occurrence of the individual learning action [7].
Another symptom of the distinction between preparation and implementation taking place is the appearance of children's words reflecting the meaning of the action. The child's emphasis on the special validity of the draft, when the pupil circles a part of the work in their notebook and signs it ‘do not evaluate’, is an indicator of how the child distinguishes and connects the two parts of the work.
We differentiate between two types of a child's behaviour. One is the construction of the ‘draft’ itself when the child's action is built up within the limits of using the means suggested by the teacher. And the second is the child's work in constructing the tool.
The psychological sense of this work is that the child is trying to determine the functional meaning. By contrasting the tool and the task in object terms (the table with ‘helpers’ and the ‘assessment table’), we have observed that for the child, a distinction between the tool and the task is not given. In the first grade, it is confusing for the child: when they take a ‘helper’, the latter is treated not as a tool but as a task. In the second grade, when children engage in making ‘helpers’, the student is confused on another level: by saying that, they are making someone a tool, they are in fact writing a task. By the end of the second grade, the child can differentiate the task from the tool. In the third grade, we encounter cases where the child takes the initiative to continue their action. For example, in the third grade, after a lesson on creating ‘helpers’, Yulia Z. asked the teacher: ‘Let me give Katya my helper, whether it can help her or not’, and then watched Katya use her helper while she worked.
Searching for one’s place of action is the initiative that in some children appears at the end of the second grade, but for the most part, it appears in the third grade as the teacher unfolds the work of creating children's helpers.
Thus, the evolution of children's learning autonomy is reflected in the ability of students to determine the extent, place and content of their training, which means that individual forms of training appear. The majority of children begin to construct an action to improve their work by addressing the table with ‘helpers’, differentiating the two parts of the work in content and scope and proactively exploring and comprehending the function of the tool, which are important indicators of the cultivation of individual learning action.
To highlight preparation as a special workspace, the teacher sets up two semantic centres in the classroom, preparation and implementation, which differ not only in content but also in the object-space form in which they are organised. Each child has a small whiteboard, there are also several small boards in the classroom which represent practice and test areas and the large board acts as a place for presenting the results. The class has a helper table, a practice table with tasks of different levels of difficulty and a separate table with quizzes.
The emphasis on special ‘places’ in the classroom reinforces the opposition between the preparation and the result. Blackboards and tables with different functional meanings act as a support for students' organisation of their action, which is constructed as a transition from one type of work to another (from preparation to realisation and vice versa). The visually presented tools (objects, diagrams, models) on the table with ‘helpers’ create a situation of choice for the child. The child can choose any tool from the variety and test it, which reinforces the content of the preparation itself.
The transition from preparation to implementation is accompanied by another polarisation of socio-institutional forms of organisation. The teaching time in primary school is divided into ‘lessons’ and ‘classes’, differing in the type of communication between the teacher and students, in the form of completion and the content of the subject material.
The type of cooperation between the teacher and the children changes in the class. The teacher in a consultant position observes the individual learning action of the child: how do they prepare the action, do they turn to ‘helpers’, do they check the completed cards and how do they decide to move on to assessment. The teacher helps to focus the child’s attention by asking: ‘How did you know that this particular card should be done?’ or ‘How did you know that you have had enough practice and it is time to move on to assessment work?’
Thus, by dividing the child's actions into preparation and implementation, the teacher assigns a specific meaning to the tool as an orientation tool right from the beginning. The teacher is engaged in constructing situations of choice so that the child can make meaningful decisions, knowing when they are ready to do something and when they are not.
Three lines of development of learning autonomy can be pointed out in learning activities: effective, exploratory and productive (as the creation of tools for theoretical thinking). These lines are based on the theory of learning activity, where there are two emphases in the learning task:
1) Discovering and modelling a general method;
2) Applying the general method to solve a class of practical problems.
Essentially, these two accents in the theory of learning activity and its practice do not follow naturally from one another. As B. Elkonin writes: ‘The learning task implies a transition from direct trial and error in achieving a result to a special construction (together with the teacher and other children) of the scaffolding of possible action (its orientational framework). Only in this transition-overcoming does the possible action itself, not just the required result, become an object of consideration, i.e. the action is re-evaluated, re-conceptualised. This is the intrigue of the learning task, and to the extent that this intrigue engages the student and is felt by them, the student transforms their own experience, i.e. proceeds to actually work with their own experience -that is, to learn’ [15, p. 30].
The two accents of the learning task were the basis for dividing the lessons into training and inquiry-based ones.
If the training lesson is aimed at teaching children how to evaluate their work, how to choose the means of overcoming deficiencies and how to work on the operational structure of the ways of action, the inquiry-based lessons unfold a child's trial of signification as a tool for understanding mathematical and linguistic relations. The result is the creation of a model for analysing and describing significant relationships.
On the transition from the individual learning activity of solving concrete practical tasks to the individual learning-research activity as a trial-and-error activity, another subject of the teacher's work appears: proactive mediation by the child. The act of mediation is subjected to a test, for example, by playing with the mathematical relations constructor, children explore limits in composing word problems (e.g. how many tasks can be compiled without extra data).
During lessons, students analyse and understand essential relationships by testing and inventing model tools—diagrams, devices, dynamic models, etc. The importance of using different signs at the same time—drawings, diagrams, tables—provides new opportunities for students to explore how transforming a mathematical relationship in one action plan (the diagram) leads to a change in another action plan (the text of the problem). In contrast to productive activity, the learning and inquiry-based activity may not be completed, because the children are involved in playing with the sign language (‘what if I put the arrows in the construction set like this?’).
The main point about the trial action is the recurrence of the child's own, original action, where the child thinks up a task scheme, addresses it to someone else and, after trying it out, comes back and reconstructs it. This recurrence is an indicator of overcoming the executive action in the trial.
Thus, an individual learning action on the result line is represented as a relationship between orientation and realisation, where the orientation is built by the child to overcome their deficits, the operational structure of the way of action is practised and the realisation is a solution to a problem.
In an individual learning activity on the theoretical line, the orientation takes on a completely different characteristic and unfolds in the trial of signification as a tool for understanding the structure of the task. The result is the creation of a model in which the student describes the essential connections and relationships they have identified. Learning and research activities are developed along the lines of modelling, which develops intensively in the third and fourth grades based on textual tasks.
Dynamics of Individual Learning Action
We suggested that the second phase of primary school age is characterised by significant changes in the development of learning autonomy, with students in third grade progressing to high levels of individual learning activities (hereafter referred to as ILE). The theoretical basis for this assumption was D. Elkonin's idea about the two phases of primary school age when from the first to the second phase, there is a transition from collaborative to individual learning activities [16]. To explore the dynamics of ILE formation on the result line of learning independence, we conducted a diagnostic procedure ‘Preparing for test’ [5, 6]. The procedure was carried out over the period of four years with students from three experimental developmental classes in which the technology of polarisation of learning space was implemented.
The purpose of the observation was to determine how the child links preparation to implementation in their work. The following observation criteria were chosen to assess the student's individual learning action: 1) choice of the type of work, reasons for choice; 2) adequacy of preparation (consistency between preparation and evaluation); 3) performance of work; 4) content of preparation: choice of practice cards in relation to own difficulties (or just easy, interesting, not difficult), independently or with help; 5) turning to the teacher; 6) turning to the tools; 7) transitions from preparation to performance and from evaluation to preparation.
The first, second, third and fourth criteria are related to goal-setting, focusing on the goal of the action and achieving the result (independent action). The fifth and sixth criteria reflect children's initiative as a search for tools. The seventh criterion is related to putting the action on hold and deciding whether to switch to assessment or to practice again (responsibility).
During the lesson, the teacher announced the option of either doing the quiz straight away or practising beforehand. The students were able to decide for themselves where to start work, at what point to move from the practice to the quiz and what tools to use in preparation.
Based on the given observation criteria, five levels of individual learning action in students have been identified. A child with a high level of Individual Learning Action can assess themselves in relation to the skills to be tested in the quiz. On this basis, the student decides whether or not they will be able to cope with the quiz. And then, they either proceed to the assessment work or choose preparation. When choosing preparation, the students demonstrate ways of overcoming their difficulties: they ask the teacher and their classmates meaningful questions; they turn to the keys or to the teacher to check the practice tasks; use ‘helpers’ which allow them to achieve higher performance levels in their quizzes. During the lesson, the student independently decides when to finish their preparation and move on to the quiz.
Table 1 presents data on the dynamics of the individual learning activities in primary school children from the first to the fourth grade.
Table 1
Dynamics of the individual learning action of primary students
ILE level |
1st grade 64 students—100% |
2nd grade 84 students—100% |
3d grade 10 students—100% |
4th grade 76 students—100% |
Low |
11 |
6 |
1 |
1 |
Below average |
9 |
5 |
6 |
7 |
Average |
69 |
74 |
62 |
46 |
Above average |
9 |
6 |
6 |
24* |
High |
2 |
9 |
25* |
22 |
Differences between classes were statistically significant using the χ2 test: * at p < 0.05.
The table shows that the transition from the first to the second grade is characterised by a decrease in the number of students in the low- and below-average groups; from the second to the third grade, there is a different trend: the number of students in the high-level group increases (by 16%). From the third to the fourth grade, the group of children with above-average levels increases significantly (by 18%).
Here’s a brief description of the qualitative changes in ILE that were observed in the experimental classes.
Firstly, the content of the training has changed significantly: the students were already able to justify their choice of work in detail and link it to the self-assessment of the skills, highlighting their difficulties. At the beginning of the second grade, only four students associated their choice of work with the tested skills, whereas, at the end of the third grade, there are 87% (73 students) could do so, and at the end of the fourth grade, there are 99% (75 students).
Secondly, we noticed a proactive approach for the students to check their work. Self-checking became internally necessary in the organisation of their preparation and was done without the request of the teacher. By the end of the third and fourth grades, checking practice cards against the keys had become the norm in preparation for quizzes.
Thirdly, by the end of third grade, there was a group of students who had a work plan to guide their preparation for the quiz (14 out of 84 students).
Thus, quantitative and qualitative analysis of the data from a four-year experimental study showed that from the second to the third grade, there was an increase in the number of students with high and above average levels of individual learning action, which supports our assumption about the dynamics of students' individual learning action in the transition from the first to the second phase of primary school age.
Conclusion
The presented experimental data have shown that the polarisation of the learning space is a prerequisite for the creation and development of learning autonomy in primary school students. It is important to emphasise that the dynamics of learning autonomy only occur if several subject areas evolve. Firstly, the evolution of the subject tools and their spatial organisation. Secondly, the evolution of the tool application situations: learning/theoretical and learning/productive, practical. Thirdly, the evolution of the socio-institutional forms.
Thus, the evolution of the activity/lesson relationship is characterised not only by the appearance of first practice lessons in the first grade and then of productive and inquiry-based lessons in the second grade but also by the appearance of the polarised lesson in the third grade, when children engage in different activities within the polarised lesson according to their interests: some make abaci—(tools for theoretical thinking), some transform different models, others practice their skills. Inside the lesson, there is a situation of choice and a free learning space within the work, where children finish with one thing and then move on to another place to do another. And in the third grade, there is a competition of investigative and productive ways for the students themselves.
Evolution proceeds in two directions:
* change of the leading form specific to each stage of training;
* the emergence of new auxiliary tools (constructors, flashcards), which, on the one hand, is the separation of the teacher with their theoretical thinking from the child, on the other hand, the separation of the child from the teacher and the appearance of the child's action as initiative, independent and responsible.
The emergence of varied forms of work: a training session—work on mistakes and skills; an inquiry-based lesson connected with constructing things and models; individual homework which the child makes for themselves; independent study of a new topic indicates that the variety of forms and their evolution quantitatively and qualitatively changes the lives of the children and the teacher.
The child's achievements in learning independence are a signal for the teacher to ‘remove’ themselves from what the child has mastered and can do now on their own. If the child can organise their training to overcome their deficits, the teacher creates the zone of proximal development for their learning autonomy (individual homework, an independent study of a new topic, etc.). Only in this case does the history of the child–adult actions as forms of agency of the child and the teacher appear.
References
- Galperin P.Ya. Psikhologiya kak ob»ektivnaya nauka [Psychology as an objective science]. Moscow: Institute of Practical Psychology, 1998. 480 p.
- Davydov V.V. Teoriya razvivayushchego obucheniya [The theory of developmental learning]. Moscow: INTOR, 1996. 544 p.
- Zaitsev S.V. Problemy razvitiya uchebnoi samostoyatel’nosti mladshikh shkol’nikov [Problems of Developing Self-Reliance in Learning in Primary School Children]. Psikhologicheskaya nauka i obrazovanie = Psychological Science and Education, 2019. Vol. 24, no. 2, pp. 50—58. DOI:10.17759/pse.2019240205 (In Russ., аbstr. in Engl.).
- Zaretsky V.K., Zaretsky Yu.V., Ostroverkh O.S., Tikhomirova A.V., Fedorenko E.Yu. Sravnitel’nyi analiz kontseptual’nykh osnovaniĭ sovremennykh obrazovatel’nykh sistem i obrazovatel’nykh praktik (na primere sravneniya sistemy razvivayushchego obucheniya i refleksivno-deyatel’nostnogo podkhoda) [A Comparative Analysis of Conceptual Bases of Modern Educational Systems and Educational Practices (on the Example of Comparison of the System of Developmental Instruction and Reflective-Activity Approach)]. Psikhologo-pedagogicheskie issledovaniya = Psychological-Educational Studies, 2020. Vol. 12, no. 4, pp. 3—18. DOI:10.17759/psyedu.2020120401
- Ostroverkh O.S., Sviridova O.I., El’konin B.D. Prostranstvo uchebnoi deyatel’nosti mladshego shkol’nika: tseli i rezul’taty [The space of educational activity of a junior schoolchild: goals and results]. Collection of materials of the 5th scientific and practical conference. “Pedagogy of development: the problem of educational results (effects)». Part 1. Krasnoyarsk, 1998, pp. 15—27.
- Ostroverkh O.S., Sviridova O.I., Mokrousova A.G. Adresnoe pedagogicheskoe deistvie kak uslovie razvitiya uchebnoi samostoyatel’nosti mladshikh shkol’nikov [Targeted pedagogical action as a condition for the development of educational independence of younger students]. Proceedings of the 12th scientific and practical conference «Pedagogy of development: the social situation of development and educational environments». Krasnoyarsk, 2006. P. 194.
- Ostroverkh O.S. Usloviya stanovleniya individual’nogo uchebnogo deistviya v obrazovatel’nom prostranstve nachal’noi shkoly [Conditions for the formation of individual educational action in the educational space of elementary school]. Nachal’naya shkola [Primary school]. 2001, no. 11, pp. 60—66.
- Polivanova K.N., Ostroverkh O.S., Strukova A.S. Predstavleniya pedagogov o detskoi samostoyatel’nosti v doshkol’nom vozraste [Representations of teachers about children’s independence in preschool age]. Sovremennoe doshkol’noe obrazovanie [Modern preschool education], 2022, no. 3(111), pp. 16—24. DOI:10.24412/2782-4519-2022-3111-16-24
- Smirnova E.O., Soldatova I.S. Osobennosti proyavleniya initsiativy sovremennykh doshkol’nikov [Features of the Initiative of Modern Preschoolers]. Psikhologo-pedagogicheskie issledovaniya = Psychological-Educational Studies, 2019. Vol. 11, no. 1, pp. 12—26. DOI:10.17759/psyedu.2019110102. (In Russ.)
- Stetsenko A.P. Kriticheskie problemy v kul’turno-istoricheskoi teorii deyatel’nosti: neotlozhnost’ sub»ektnosti [Critical Challenges in Cultural-Historical Activity Theory: the Urgency of Agency]. Кul’turno-istoricheskaya psikhologiya = Cultural-Historical Psychology, 2020. Vol. 16, no. 2, pp. 5—18. DOI:10.17759/chp.2020160202
- Tsukerman G.A., Venger A.L. Razvitie uchebnoi samostoyatel’nosti sredstvami shkol’nogo obrazovaniya [Development of educational autonomy by means of school education]. Psikhologicheskaya nauka i obrazovanie = Psychological science and education, 2010. Vol. 15, no. 4, pp. 77—90.
- Frumin I.D., Elkonin B.D. Obrazovatel’noe prostranstvo kak prostranstvo razvitiya («shkola vzrosleniya») [Educational space as a development space («school of growing up»)]. Voprosy psikhologii [Questions of Psychology], 1993, no. 1, pp. 24—32.
- Elkonin B.D. Oposredstvovanie. Deistvie. Razvitie. [Mediation. Action. Development.] Izhevsk: ERGO, 2010. 280 p.
- Elkonin B.D. L.S. Vygotskii — D.B. El’konin: znakovoe oposredstvovanie i sovokupnoe deistvie [L.S. Vygotsky-D.B. Elkonin: semiotic mediation and cumulative action]. Voprosy psikhologii [Questions of Psychology], 1996, no. 5, pp. 57—63.
- Elkonin B.D. Sovremennost’ teorii i praktiki Uchebnoi Deyatel’nosti: klyuchevye voprosy i perspektivy [The Modernity of the Theory and Practice of Educational Activities: Key Issues and Prospects]. Psikhologicheskaya nauka i obrazovanie = Psychological Science and Education, 2020. Vol. 25, no. 4, pp. 28—39. DOI:10.17759/pse.2020250403
- Elkonin D.B. Izbrannye psikhologicheskie trudy [Selected psychological works]. Moscow: Pedagogy, 1989. 560 p.
- Andreas Schleicher. Helping our Youngest to Learn and Grow: Policies for Early Learning, International Summit on the Teaching Profession. Paris: OECD Publishing, 2019. DOI:10.1787/9789264313873-en
- Daniels H., Cole M. & Wertsch J. (Eds.). The Cambridge Companion to Vygotsky. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 2007. DOI:10.1017/CCOL0521831040
- Engeness I. Teacher facilitating of group learning in science with digital technology and insights into students’ agency in learning to learn. Research in science & technological education, 2020, no. 1(38), pp. 42—62.
- Fay D. & Frese M. The nature of personal initiative: Self-starting orientation and proactivity. Paper presented at the annual conference of the Soceity for Industrial and Organizational Psychology, 1998. Dallas, TX
- Frese M., Fay D., Hilburger T., Leng K. & Tag A. The concept of personal initiative: Operationalization, reliability and validity of two German samples. Journal of Occupational and Organizational Psychology, 1997. Vol. 70(2), pp. 139—161. DOI:10.1111/j.2044-8325.1997.tb00639.x
- OECD Student agency for 2030 [Book]. Paris: OECD Publicing, 2019.
- Perpetua K. Children’s Agency in the Modern Primary Classroom. CHILDREN & SOCIETY, 2020. Vol. 34, Iss. 3, pp. 17—30. DOI:10.1111/chso.12357
- Rebecca D., Galinsky E. [et al.] Autonomy-supportive parenting and associations with child and parent executive function. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 2018. Vol. 58, pp. 77—85.
- Reedy A., De Carvalho R. Children’s perspectives on reading, agency and their environment: what can we learn about reading for pleasure from an East London primary school? Education 3—13, 2021, no. 2(49), pp. 134—147.
- Reeve J. Teachers as Facilitators: What autonomy-supportive teachers do and why their students benefit [Journal]. Chicago: Chicago Press, 2006. Vol. 106, no. 3, pp. 225—236. DOI:10.1086/501484
- Sevtap G., Emma S. Children’s agency in parent—child, teacher—pupil and peer relationship contexts. International Journal of Qualitative Studies on Health and Well-being, 2018. Vol. 13, рр. 1565239. DOI:10.1080/17482631.2019.1565239
- Taub M. [et al.]. The agency effect: The impact of student agency on learning, emotions, and problem-solving behaviors in a game-based learning environment. Computers & Education, 2020. Vol. 147, рр. 103781.
- Yasnitsky A., Van der Veer R. & Ferrari M. (Eds.). The Cambridge Handbook of Cultural-Historical Psychology (Cambridge Handbooks in Psychology). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 2014. DOI:10.1017/CBO9781139028097
Information About the Authors
Metrics
Views
Total: 979
Previous month: 16
Current month: 20
Downloads
Total: 341
Previous month: 7
Current month: 9