Relationship Between Personality Traits and Emotion Regulation Among Adolescent Athletes

277

Abstract

Emotions are inevitable in our day-to-day life and play most important role in maintaining interpersonal relationship, communication, and overall quality of life. The ability to regulate emotion is also very important to deal effectively with everyday life stressors. Personality is an important aspect of human being and determines the unique adjustment of human being with his/her environment. Personality traits also influence the perception of an individual and thereby impact the way an individual regulates his/her emotions. Emotion regulation varies significantly across individuals so as the personality traits. The present study aims at determining the relationship between personality traits and emotion regulation among athletes and non-athletes. The present cross- sectional study was conducted on participants (N=180), consisting of athletes (N=80; Male= 55 and Female= 25) and non-athletes (N= 100; Male= 45 and Female= 55). All participants were selected through convenient sampling method. Pearson’s product moment correlation of coefficient reveals that for athletes Openness to experience and extraversion are significantly and positively correlated to cognitive reappraisal and neuroticism is significantly and positively correlated to expressive suppression. This means that athletes high on openness to experience and extraversion tend to use cognitive appraisal as an emotion regulation strategy whereas athletes high on neuroticism tend to prefer suppression strategy. For non-athletes, openness to experience, conscientiousness are significantly and positively correlated to cognitive reappraisal and neuroticism is significantly and positively related to suppression. This suggests that individuals high on openness to experience and conscientiousness are also high on cognitive appraisal and those who are high on neuroticism are also high suppression. Multiple regression analyses suggest for athletes, openness to experience and neuroticism are the significant predictors of Cognitive reappraisal and neuroticism is the most significant predictor of expressive suppression. For non-athletes, conscientiousness and openness to experience are the significant predictors of Cognitive reappraisal. and neuroticism is the most significant predictor of expressive suppression. The study discusses the pertinent potential implications of results.

General Information

Keywords: athletes, big five factor, cognitive reappraisal, neuroticism, suppression

Journal rubric: Developmental Psychology

Article type: scientific article

DOI: https://doi.org/10.17759/pse.2023280207

Acknowledgements. The authors are grateful to all participants took part in this study and to Amity University Kolkata.

Received: 23.04.2022

Accepted:

For citation: Karmakar R., Ghosh A. Relationship Between Personality Traits and Emotion Regulation Among Adolescent Athletes. Psikhologicheskaya nauka i obrazovanie = Psychological Science and Education, 2023. Vol. 28, no. 2, pp. 83–94. DOI: 10.17759/pse.2023280207.

Full text

Introduction

A meaningful life consists of close interpersonal relationships, and understanding emotions is pivotal as they govern our actions, thoughts, and relationships. The word “emotion” originated in the Rasa theory of ancient Indian literature. Rasa means ‘juice’ or ‘flavour’, which refers to the different feelings aroused by the ancient Indian arts. Some Sanskrit texts such as bhāva (feeling) and in particular rāga (love, attraction), dveṣa (hatred, aversion), harṣa (joy), bhaya (fear) and śoka (sorrow) elucidated the term “emotion”.  Cognition (jñāna) and emotions are interrelated, and their relationship affects an individual’s behavioral aspects. The interrelationship among Affect (emotion), Cognition and Behaviour may be represented by the well-known ABC model in psychology shown in Figure 1.

Figure 1. ABC model (Triad) in psychology, Ostrom [32].

Emotion regulation is crucial to maintaining a healthy mental equilibrium, as inefficient regulation can be detrimental to our mental health. Emotion regulation as the ability to regulate one’s own emotions and to express emotional responses effectively. Successful emotion regulation plays a vital role in maintaining  mental health [2], while ineffective emotion regulation may lead to affective disorders [34], anxiety [26] and long-term substance use[12]. The current study focuses on two emotion regulation strategies, namely cognitive reappraisal and suppression. Cognitive reappraisal, or adaptive emotion regulation, or antecedent focused strategy uses cognitive resources to modify the thought process and thereby change emotional impact. It helps to perceive emotion provoking situations as more positive and non-threatening, which is beneficial to mental health and thereby promotes sustainability in the long term. On the other hand, suppression or maladaptive emotion regulation or a focus on reactivity (suppressing the behavioral expression of emotions) may hamper mental health and thus reduce the likelihood of resilience. Therefore, cognitive reappraisal can effectively regulate negative emotional reactions compared to suppression [5, 9].

Research evidence on Emotion Regulation

Behavioral studies [6, 10, 14, 15, 17, 19] have found that reappraisal is more effective than suppression. This provides indirect evidence that antecedent-oriented strategies are generally more effective in regulating emotions than response-focused strategies [37, 38]. Reappraisal decreases the emotional experience and reduces negative expressive behaviour [33].  Emotion regulation is a skill which shifts over time and these changes are more visible in adulthood because adulthood is a life stage full of challenges and hardships. Emotion regulation goals are closely related to situational factors and vary across different events within the same individual. For achieving pre-hedonic goals, emotion regulation strategies that are effective in altering emotional experience are crucial. However, expressive suppression can be useful for attaining social goals [43]. Emotion regulation also varies with respect to the nature of the situation. During competition, athletes experience high level of positive and negative emotions that are generated by spectators and situational demands, so emotional regulation has become crucial in the field of sport [18, 21]. Furthermore, it has been observed that athletes perceive cognitive reappraisal as an adaptive strategy in terms of well-being and efficacy in regulating stressful situations during competition, as this strategy helps them better adapt to the situation by enhancing positive emotions [29, 40], while the opposite effect is observed in case of emotional suppression [30, 40]. Martinent et al. [24] suggested that high perceived efficacy leads to automaticity in emotion regulation. Automatic strategies corresponded to several specific emotions, such as anger, joy and relief. They stated that positive emotions are well controlled in most circumstances, compared to negative emotions such as anxiety, discouragement and disgust, as there is greater variability in the effectiveness of these emotions.

Personality is another important factor in explaining human behaviour. In ancient Indian literature personality (prakriti) is considered from two different perspectives — biological and psychological. The biological perspective is viewed in Ayurveda, an ancient medical scripture, and psychological perspective is considered in Tri-gunas such as Sattva, Rajas and Tamas. The meaning of being sattvic is associated with strength of character, firmness, courage and generosity. Sattva is characterized by idealism, analyticism, intelligence, prudence, lack of selfishness, self-control, extrovertedness, malleability and conscientiousness. Rajasic guna seems to be related to emotion-driven, self-oriented traits such as openness to experience and emotional stability. The meaning of tamas is exhaustion, depression, rigidity, mental imbalance and neuroticism. Although the origin of Triguna can be found in Bhagavad Gita, it still explains the understanding of individuals’ psyche. Moreover, Personality plays an important role in goal setting and an athlete needs to set goal/s before performance and then work out a certain strategy to reach the goal/s. Hence the study of personality is also important in sports research.

Research evidence on Personality

Research indicate that athletes do not differ from non-athletes with regard to the extraversion personality trait [4]. Butt [8], Cox [11], Saint-Phard [35] reported that the competitive athlete presents some psychological characteristics that distinguish him from other populations. Among these differences, the authors consider that athletes have higher emotional stability, extroversion, self-confidence and higher mental stability compared to non-athletes. Maresh et al. [23] compared a group of runners with a group of non-athletes and noted that these athletes were more withdrawn, thoughtful and presented lower anger levels than non-athletes. Weinberg and Gould [42] and Hernández Ardieta et al. [16] demonstrated that the athletes are more aggressive, independent, emotionally more stable and more work-focused than non-athletes. Weinberg and Gould [42] reported that team athletes are more extroverted. Piepiora [32] reported that team sport champions are characterized by a lower level of neuroticism, a higher level of extraversion and openness to experience than other sportsmen. Morgan and Costill [27] reported that players who succeed in team sports tend to show a higher level of extraversion, openness to experiences and a lower level of neuroticismcompared to other sportsmen. Nia and Besharat [28] suggested that athletes who played individual sports have higher scores on conscientiousness and autonomy compared to those who played team sports. However, athletes who played team sports scored significantly higher on sociotropy and agreeableness. They reported no significant differences between two groups on domains such as neuroticism, extraversion and openness. Wang, Shi and Li [41] found that there is a relationship between extraversion, neuroticism and positive and negative emotions, mediated by the contribution of reappraisal and suppression. Extraversion indicators such as warm-heartedness, assertiveness, vigor, positive emotions have relationship with emotion regulation [22]. Lane et al. [20] suggested that players often use strategies to reinforce unpleasant emotions, yet an increased state of anger frequently improves performance, while an elevated state of anxiety interferes.

Research evidence on Personality and Emotional Regulation

Tao et al. [39] conducted a study on male professional firefighters in China and revealed that conscientiousness through cognitive reappraisal can reduce anxiety and depression symptoms among Chinese firefighters. Alcaraz-Ibáñez et al. [1] conducted a study examining Big Five personality traits and body-related self-conscious emotions among Spanish undergraduate students, and the results showed that neuroticism, extraversion, conscientiousness and openness to experience were significant predictors of body-related self-conscious emotions. Hamzah et al. [ 36] conducted a study on Malaysian elite and non-elite tennis players, and the findings demonstrated that elite and non-elite tennis players scored the highest on the Agreeableness indicator. Moreover, the study showed that female tennis players had higher scores on Neuroticism than their male counterparts. The study also revealed that the Big Five personality traits can have a great influence on tennis sport. Brito et al. [7] carried out a study on boxing athletes’ mental toughness and physical fitness performance using Big Five Factors. The results showed that Neuroticism was related to mental toughness, and agreeableness was related to muscular endurance capability. The study concluded that mental toughness and endurance were correlated with boxing athletes’.

Based on the studies presented above, it may be observed that there are several differences between athletes and non-athletes with respect to personality traits. However, there is a lack of researches stating the relationship between emotional regulation and personality traits among athletes and non-athletes. Studying emotion regulation is very important for athletes before and during competitions. Another uniqueness of the present study is that it was conducted during the COVID-19 outbreak, when people experience uncertainty in their daily lives and radical shift from normal to abnormal situation. This radical shift from a routine schedule to a completely new schedule has created many problems in their daily lives, as the uncertainty factor prevailed in this time frame. While dealing with these problems, people are confronting new emotional perspectives, and sometimes it becomes very difficult for them to control and regulate their emotions in a desired and socially acceptable way.

The objectives of the present study are to determine:

1.     The relationship between Personality traits (neuroticism, extraversion, openness to experience, agreeableness and conscientiousness) and emotion regulation (Cognitive reappraisal and suppression).

2.     Significant differences between athletes and non-athletes with respect to personality traits (neuroticism, extraversion, openness to experience, agreeableness and conscientiousness) and emotion regulation (Cognitive reappraisal and suppression).

3.     Significant predictor/s (if any) of emotion regulation among athletes and non-athletes.

Method

A. Participants

The convenience sampling method was used to collect data from a sample of 180 individuals, among whom 80 were athletes (Male= 55 and Female= 25) and 100 were non-athletes (Male= 45 and Female= 55). The athletes were aged 13- 18 years (M = 15.75 years; SD = 3.45) and the non-athletes were aged 13-19 years (16.94 years, SD= 2.82). The present study included professional athletes who The sample comprised of individuals residing in West Bengal, India and belonging to the middle socio-economic status. The athlete group played collective sports such as cricket, soccer and volleyball.

B. Tools Used

  1. NEO-FFI-3 form S [25]: NEO-FFI-3 form S was administered for measuring the five main personality dimensions of the Big Five factor model (neuroticism, extraversion, openness to experience, agreeableness and conscientiousness). It is suitable for respondents aged 12 and over. Higher scores on each dimension indicate high scores on the respective personality dimensions. The participants were asked to answer 60 items using a 5-point Likert scale ranging from 0 (strongly disagree) to 4 (strongly agree). The internal consistency coefficients of the different NEO-FFI-3 dimensions ranged from 0.75 to 0.83 for the five scales. Neuroticism (N) is an indicator for the degree of emotional instability, lack of impulse control and anxiety. People with high N score are considered to have a higher incidence of distress, low adaptivity, irrational ideas, negative feelings and low self-esteem. Extraversion (E) is an expression of sociability and assertiveness. Extraverts tend to enjoy gathering and working in groups, have a cheerful disposition and are optimistic. Openness to experience (O) reflects a propensity for intellectual curiosity, innovation and open-mindedness. Individuals with high scores for this domain are regarded as having less conventional behavior. Agreeableness (A) is characterized by altruism, helpfulness, sympathy for others, trust in others’ intentions and respect for others’ beliefs, and high scores on this domain indicate cooperativeness. People with a high Conscientiousness (C) score are generally purposeful, organized, punctual, determined, trustworthy, and achievement-oriented.

  2. Emotion Regulation Questionnaire (ERQ): Developed by Gross & John [15], this 10-item self-report questionnaire is based on Gross’s [13] model of the emotion regulation process. The ERQ is designed to measure people’s usage of two regulation strategies: an antecedent-oriented strategy called cognitive reappraisal and a response-focused strategy called suppression. Cognitive reappraisal consists of 6 items, where a person attempts to modify the way he or she thinks about a situation in order to change its emotional impact (e.g., “When I’m faced with a stressful situation, I make myself think about it in a way that helps me stay calm”). Suppression includes 4 items, where a person tries to inhibit the behavioral expression of his or her emotions (e.g., “I keep my emotions to myself”). Separate scale scores are derived for these two regulation strategies. All items are answered on a 7-point Likert scale, ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 7 (strongly agree), with higher scores indicating greater usage of that strategy. The Cronbach’s alphas for cognitive reappraisal and suppression are 0.78 and 0.71, respectively.

C. Procedure 

The convenience sampling method was used to collect data from a sample of 180 individuals via Google form. Consent was obtained by explaining the purpose of the study to participants and assuring that they had the right to withdraw from the study anytime during the entire process. The participants were guaranteed confidentiality and were therefore requested to answer as objectively as possible. The participants were informed that there was no time limit, but it would be preferable if they answered the statements quickly and did not ponder upon each statement for a long time. Finally, each participant was thanked for his/her cooperation.

Results and Discussion

The raw data collected via Google Form (online) were validated and analyzed using SPSS 21. The means, standard deviations (SD) and Pearson’s product moment correlation of coefficients were calculated between personality traits and emotion regulation among athletes and non-athletes and are presented in Tables 1 and 2.

Table 1

Descriptive statistics and correlations between measures of Big Five Factor Traits and Emotional Regulation of Athletes (N= 80)

 

Mean (SD)

1

2

3

4

5

6

7

1. Extraversion

25.09 (2.09)

1

 

 

 

 

 

 

2. Agreeableness

29.52 (6.10)

.09

1

 

 

 

 

 

3. Openness to experience

30.18 (3.04)

.11

.10

1

 

 

 

 

4. Conscientiousness

33.76 (4.09)

-.27*

.30**

.10

1

 

 

 

5. Neuroticism

25.93 (3.04)

-.10

-.22*

-.25*

-.39**

1

 

 

6. Cognitive reappraisal

32.54 (3.91)

.32**

.10

.41**

.09

-.35**

1

 

7. Suppression

16.58 (9.31)

-.26*

.09

-.31**

.10

.32**

-.39**

1

**p<0.01 level of significance, *p<0.05 level of significance

Table 2

Descriptive statistics and correlations between measures of Big Five Factor Traits and Emotional Regulation of Non- Athletes (N= 100)

 

Mean (SD)

1

2

3

4

5

6

7

1. Extraversion

25.80 (3.17)

1

 

 

 

 

 

 

2. Agreeableness

28.27 (2.99)

.04

1

 

 

 

 

 

3. Openness to experience

27.17 (4.41)

-.15

.13

1

 

 

 

 

4. Conscientiousness

28.26 (2.26)

.10

.38**

.11

1

 

 

 

5. Neuroticism

27.62 (3.45)

.08

-.19

-.24*

-.36**

1

 

 

6. Cognitive reappraisal

28.90 (4.03)

.08

.10

.33**

.39**

-.10

1

 

7. Suppression

18.29 (2.91)

.10

.07

-.11

.08

.29**

-.27**

1

**p<0.01 level of significance, *p<0.05 level of significance

Tables 1 and 2 represent that athletes’ openness to experience and extraversion are significantly and positively correlated to cognitive reappraisal, while neuroticism is significantly and negatively related to cognitive reappraisal. Openness to experience and extraversion are significantly and negatively related to suppression, while neuroticism is significantly and positively related to suppression. For athletes, extraversion is negatively related to conscientiousness, whereas a significant positive relationship is evident between agreeableness and conscientiousness. Neuroticism is found to be significantly and negatively related to agreeableness, openness to experience and conscientiousness.

For non-athletes, openness to experience and conscientiousness are significantly and positively correlated to cognitive reappraisal, while neuroticism is significantly and positively related to suppression. Agreeableness is significantly and positively correlated to conscientiousness. Openness to experience and conscientiousness are significantly and negatively correlated to neuroticism. It may be due to the fact that individuals with high conscientiousness, openness to experience and extraversion often modify situations positively, accept others’ opinions, are energetic and enthusiastic, and hence are better able to manage emotions effectively and maintain good mental health. On the other hand, Neurotic individuals are emotionally less stable, find it difficult to restructure their emotions and, as a result, are unable to regulate their emotions. An inability to regulate emotions effectively may lead to emotional suppression and worsening mental health.

Independent t-tests were calculated to determine significant differences between athletes and non-athletes with regard to personality traits and emotional regulation and are presented in Table 3.

Table 3.

Mean, Standard Deviation (SD) and t-value of Big Five Factor traits and emotional regulation among athletes and non-athletes.

Variables

Athletes

Non-athletes

t values

Mean (SD)

Mean (SD)

Extraversion

25.09 (2.09)

25.80 (3.17)

1.73

Agreeableness

29.52 (6.10)

28.27 (2.99)

1.79

Openness to experience

30.18 (3.04)

27.17 (4.41)

5.20**

Conscientiousness

33.76 (4.09)

28.26 (2.26)

11.44**

Neuroticism

25.93 (3.04)

27.62 (3.45)

3.44**

Cognitive reappraisal

32.54 (3.91

28.90 (4.03)

6.10**

Suppression

16.58 (9.31)

18.29 (2.91)

1.74

**p<0.01 level of significance

Table 3 represents that athletes are significantly higher on Openness to experience and Conscientiousness compared to non-athletes, whereas the opposite is true for Neuroticism. In case of emotional regulation, athletes are significantly higher on Cognitive reappraisal compared to their non-athletes counterparts. It may be due to the fact that conscientiousness, such as perseverance and diligence, as well as the ability to manage stress and emotions (emotional stability) are crucial to an athlete’s performance.  In addition to conscientiousness, Openness to experience, characterized by flexibility, creativity, acceptance of other people's ideas, is also very important, especially for team sports. This finding is favored by researchers [3, 44] who suggest that a higher level of openness may play a greater role in predicting participation in non-traditional sports due to the level of receptivity to ideas and opportunities for new experiences. Regarding emotion regulation, athletes are significantly higher on Cognitive reappraisal compared to their non-athletes counterparts. The likely reason for this is that athletes need to effectively manage their emotions in order to concentrate on performance, as suppressing emotion can interfere with concentration and thereby have a detrimental effect on their performance. Athletes need to have emotional balance, effective team communication and tactical thinking skills that affect their readiness to compete [16].

In order to determine the significant predictor/s of emotion regulation, multiple regression analyses (separately for athletes and non-athletes) were carried out and presented in the following tables:                           

Table 4a.

Regression analysis of dimensions of the Big Five personality traits on the athletes’ emotion regulation

Criterion

Predictors

B (Unstandardized coefficients)

???? Standardized coefficients

t value

Model 1: Cognitive reappraisal

 

Openness to experience

1.58

0.30

 

6.15**

Neuroticism

 

-1.57

-0.26

5.61**

Model 2: Suppression

Neuroticism

-1.45

-0.25

4.94**

Note: Model 1.   ???? 2 = 0.39, Adjusted ????2 = 0.38 ** p<0.01 level,

Model 2. ????2 = 0.18, Adjusted ????2 = 0.17

Table 4b.

Regression analysis of dimensions of the Big Five personality traits on the non-athletes’ emotion regulation

Criterion

Predictors

B (Unstandardized coefficients)

???? Standardized coefficients

t value

Model 1: Cognitive reappraisal

 

Conscientiousness

0.62

0.12

2.55**

Openness to experience

2.16

0.59

4.55**

Model 2: Suppression

Neuroticism

-0.54

-0.10

2.10*

Model 1. ????2 = 0.43, Adjusted ????2 = 0.42, ** p<0.01 level

Model 2. ????2 = 0.19, Adjusted ????2 = 0.18, * p<0.05 level

Table 4a shows that for athletes, openness to experience and neuroticism are the significant predictors of Cognitive reappraisal. Adjusted ????2 is found to be 0.38, indicating that openness to experience and neuroticism explain 38% of the variance in cognitive reappraisal. The second model represents that neuroticism is the most significant predictor of expressive suppression. Adjusted ????2 is found to be 0.17, suggesting that neuroticism explains 17% of the variance in expressive suppression. Table 4b reveals that for non-athletes, conscientiousness and openness to experience are the significant predictors of Cognitive reappraisal. Adjusted ????2 is found to be 0.42, indicating that conscientiousness and openness to experience explain 42% of the variance in cognitive reappraisal. The second model represents that neuroticism is the most significant predictor of expressive suppression. Adjusted ????2 is found to be 0.18, suggesting that neuroticism explains 18% of the variance in expressive suppression.

The intriguing finding of regression analysis is that people who are ready to accept new ideas, concepts and actions tend to reappraise an emotional situation by altering its meaning and emotional impact, whereas people experiencing frequent negative emotional states tend to suppress emotions and thereby are more likely to experience low self-esteem, anxiety and reduced well-being in the long run.

Conclusion

Concentration is a desideratum for athletes to perform effectively in sport. To maintain good mental health, athletes tend to use cognitive resources to modify the thought process. This study concludes that athletes are significantly higher on the Openness to experience and Conscientiousness dimension of personality compared to non-athletes, while neuroticism tends to be inverted. Interestingly, the study shows that open-minded people are flexible by nature, tend to experience new things and relationships in life, and prefer to reinterpret an emotionally arousing situation in a way that changes the emotional impact and alters its meaning. On the other hand, emotionally unstable people are not capable of cognitive re-interpretation and tend to hide, inhibit or reduce ongoing emotional expression. For non-athletes, conscientiousness is significantly and positively correlated to cognitive reappraisal and neuroticism is significantly and positively related to expressive suppression. It can therefore be concluded that people who control their impulses tend to be goal-directed and, consequently, reframe the meaning of emotion-laden situations in an effective way. Openness to experience and conscientiousness are the significant predictors of Cognitive reappraisal. The most significant predictor of expressive suppression is neuroticism.

Limitations and Implications

The present study has limitations. First, we cannot draw causal inferences from the results because of the cross-sectional design. Longitudinal and sequential research design would be helpful. The second limitation is that responses are self-reported and collected online. Future research should replicate these findings using other offline methods (e.g., observations and face-to-face interviews, etc.). The third limitation of this study is the relatively small sample size which is also location specific (West Bengal district only). Further study based on samples selected from wider regional/cultural backgrounds may also be useful. The fourth limitation could be the lack of knowledge about the extent to which the specificity of sports disciplines and training influences an athlete’s personality development. Last but not least, due to time and situational constraints, the study has not included other variables that directly influence emotion regulation, such as psychological and social capital, etc.

Despite these limitations, the present study has implications for our daily lives. Emotion regulation has proved to be a crucial strategy for maintaining a positive climate in our daily lives, especially in the current situation. The study highlights the need to introduce counselling in academic institutions to make students aware of the pros and cons of different emotion regulation strategies in real life situations. Critical evaluation of such strategies would ameliorate students’ decision to utilize them to deal with stressful situations in future, thereby promoting well-being, social connectedness, academic performance and sustainability in the long run. Educational institutions must include emotion regulation interventions (such as psychoeducation, emotional awareness and the introduction of behavioural science) as a part of the curriculum.  The study is relevant in the field of team sports, as emotion regulation is generated by both players and other teammates. Sports psychologists and counsellors need to be appointed in teaching the ability to cognitively reinterpret a situation so that athletes learn strategies to reduce additional anxiety and stress during performance and refocus on the competition. Effective emotion regulation also increases athletes’ acceptance of any situation, which in turn proliferates their adaptability in life. Understanding one’s own personality traits gives insight to the individuals and also helps them comprehend others better, leading to stronger intra– and interpersonal relationships. This understanding helps an individual to evolve and shape him/herself into a better person and adopt an appropriate emotion regulation strategy to deal effectively with the hardships of life.

References

  1. Alcaraz-Ibáñez M., Sicilia A., Paterna A. Examining the associations between the Big Five personality traits and body self-conscious emotions. Psych J. 2020. Vol. 9(3) pp. 392-401. DOI:10.1002/pchj.324
  2. Aldao A., Nolen-Hoeksema S., Schweizer S. Emotion-regulation strategies across psychopathology: A meta-analytic review. Clinical Psychology Review, Vol. 30, pp. 217-237. DOI:10.1016/j.cpr.2009.11.004
  3. Allen M.S., Greenlees I., Jones M.V. Personality in sport: A comprehensive review. International Review of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 2013, Vol. 6, pp. 184-208. DOI: 10.1080/1750984X.2013.769614
  4. Auweele Y.V., Nys K., Rzewnicki R., Mele V. Personality and athlete. In: Singer R, Haussenblas HA, Janelle CM, editors. Handbook of sport psychology. New York: John Wiley & Sons, 2001, pp. 239-268.
  5. Bai X., Jia L., Wang J. Emotional priming effects on difficult stroop task for trait anxiety. Psychol. Sci, 2015. Vol. 38, pp. 1032–1038. DOI:10.16719/j.cnki.1671-6981.20160102
  6. Bebko F., Franconeri S.L., Ochsner K.N., Chiao Y. Look before you regulate: differential perceptual strategies underlying expressive suppression and cognitive reappraisal. Emotion, 2011. Vol. 11, pp. 732–742. DOI: 10.1037/a0024009
  7. Brito C.J, Chagas A.L.S, Brito M.A., Müller V.T., Noronha A.S., Coswig V., Teixeira F.G., Soto D.A.S., Miarka B. Mental toughness and physical fitness tests of boxing athletes associated with big five personality factors. Revista Brasileira de Cineantropometria e Desempenho Humano, 2023. 25(6). DOI:10.1590/1980-0037.2023v25e87135
  8. Butt D.S. Personality of the athlete. In: Butt DS, editor. The psychology of sport. New York: VNR. 1987
  9. Cheng L., Yuan J., He Y., Li H. Emotion regulation strategies: Cognitive reappraisal is more effective than expressive suppression. Psychol. Sci, 2009. Vol. 17, pp. 730–735.
  10. Cheng L., Yuan, J.J., Yuan-Yuan H.E., Hong L.I. Emotion Regulation Strategies: Cognitive Reappraisal Is More Effective than Expressive Suppression. Advances in Psychological Science, 2009. Vol. 17, pp. 730–735. DOI: 10.3389/fnsys.2014.00175
  11. Cox R.H. Sport psychology: concepts and applications, 1994. 2nd ed. Dubuque: Brown & Benchmark.
  12. Fox H.C., Axelrod S.R., Paliwal P., Sleeper J., Sinha R. Difficulties in emotion regulation and impulse control during cocaine abstinence. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 2007. Vol. 89, pp. 298−301. DOI: 1016/j.drugalcdep.2006.12.026
  13. Gross J. J. Antecedent- and response-focused emotion regulation: divergent consequences for experience, expression, and physiology. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 1998. Vol. 74, pp. 224–237. DOI: 1037/0022-3514.74.1.224
  14. Gross J. J. Emotion regulation in adulthood: Timing is everything. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 2001. Vol. 10, pp. 214–219.DOI:10.1111/1467-8721.00152
  15. Gross J. J., John, O. P. Individual differences in two emotion regulation processes: implication for affect, relationships, and well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 2003. Vol. 85, pp. 348–362. DOI:1037/0022-3514.85.2.348
  16. Hernández-Ardieta I.P., Lopez J.C., Dolores M., Ruiz E.J.G. Personalidad, diferencias individuales y ejecución deportiva. In: Zafra A, Ruiz HJ, Garcia GN, coordenadores. Manual de psicología del deporte. 2002. Murcia: DM, pp. 105-123.
  17. Hofmann S. G., Heering S., Sawyer A. T., Asnaani A. How to handle anxiety: The effects of reappraisal, acceptance, and suppression strategies on anxious arousal. Behaviour Research & Therapy, 2009. Vol. 47, pp. 389–394. DOI:1016/j.brat.2009.02.010
  18. Jones M. V. Emotion regulation and performance. In: S. Murphy (Ed.), The Oxford handbook of sport and performance psychology, 2012 (pp. 154-72). New York: Oxford University Press. DOI: 10.1093/oxfordhb/9780199731763.013.0008
  19. Kalokerinos E. K., Greenaway K. H., Denson T. F. Reappraisal but not suppression downregulates the experience of positive and negative emotion. Emotion, 2015. Vol. 15, pp. 271-275.  DOI: 10.1037/emo0000025
  20. Lane, A. M., Beedie, C. J., Devonport, T. J., and Stanley, D.M. Instrumental emotion regulation in sport: relationships between beliefs about emotion and emotion regulation strategies used by athletes. Scandinavian Journal of Medicine & Science in Sports, 2011.21, no. 6 : e445-e451.
  21. Lane A., Beedie C., Jones M. V., Uphill M., Devonport T. The bases expert statement on emotion regulation in sport. Journal of Sport Sciences, 2012. Vol. 30, no. 11, pp. 1189- 1195. DOI:10.1080/02640414.2012.693621
  22. Livingstone K.M., Srivastava, S. Personality and positive emotion.In M.M. Tugade, M. N. Shiota, L. D. Kirby (Eds.), Handbook of positive emotions, 2014. . The Guilford Press. 82–99.
  23. Maresh C.M., Sheckley B.G., Allen G.J., Camaione D.N., Sinatra S.T. Middle age male distances runners: physiological and psychological profiles. Journal of Sports Med Phys Fitness, 1991. Vol. 31, pp. 461-469. PMID: 1798323 
  24. Martinent G., Ledos S., Ferrand C., Campo M., Nicolas M.. Athletes’ regulation of emotions experienced during competition: A naturalistic video-assisted study. Sport, Exercise, and Performance Psychology, 2015.4, no. 3, pp. 188-205. DOI:10.1037/spy0000037
  25. McCrae, R.R., and Costa, P.T.Jr. NEO Inventories Professional Manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.
  26. Mennin D.S., Holaway R., Fresco D.M., Moore M.T., Heimberg R.G. Delineating components, emotion and its dysregulation in anxiety and mood psychopathology. Behaviour Therapy, 2007. Vol. 38, pp. 284–302. DOI:10.1016/j.beth.2006.09.001
  27. Morgan, W.P., and Costill, D.L. Selected psychological characteristics and health behaviors of aging marathon runners: a longitudinal study. International journal of sports medicine17, no. 04 (1996): pp. 305-312. DOI: 1055/s-2007-972852
  28. Nia M. E, Besharat M.A. Comparison of athletes’ personality characteristics in individual and team sports. Procedia-Social and Behavioral Sciences,2010. 5, pp. 808-812. DOI:10.1016/j.sbspro.2010.07.189
  29. Oriol X., Filella G., Gomila, M. Regulación emocional de los resultados adversos en competición: estrategias funcionales en deportes colectivos. Cuadernos de Psicología del Deporte, 2014. Vol. 14, no. 1, pp. 63-72. DOI:10.4321/S1578-84232014000100008
  30. Oriol X., Molina V., DaCosta S., Páez D. Regulación Afectiva y Bienestar en Ámbito Deportivo: Validación de las escalas de heterorregulación y su relación con bienestar y eficacia colectiva. In: J.C., Oyanedel.; M., Bilbao., y D., Páez (Ed.), La felicidad de los chilenos: Estudio sobre bienestar, 2015,. Santiago: RIL Editores. pp. 49-66 .
  31. OstromM.The relationship between the affective, behavioral, and cognitive components of attitude.  Journal of experimental Exp. Soc. Psychol, 1969.  Vol. 5, No. 1, pp. 12-30. DOI:10.1037/0022-3514.59.2.202
  32. Piepiora P. Assessment of personality traits influencing the performance of men in team sports in terms of the big five. Frontiers in Psychology, 2021. Vol. 12, 679724. DOI:10.3389/fpsyg.2021.679724
  33. Richards J.M., Gross J.J. Emotion regulation and memory: the cognitive costs of keeping one’s cool. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 2000. Vol. 79, pp. 410–424. DOI:10.1002/9781118133880.hop205018
  34. Rottenberg J., Gross J.J., Wilhelm F.H., Najmi S., Gotlib I.H. Crying threshold and intensity in major depressive disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 2005. Vol. 111, 2, pp. 302–312. PMID:12003451
  35. Saint-Phard D., Van Dorsten B., Marx R.G., York K.A. Self-perception in elite collegiate female gymnastics, cross-country runners and track-and-field athletes. Mayo Clin Proc, 1999. Vol. 74, pp. 770-774. DOI:4065/74.8.770
  36. Sharifah R.S.H., Mohamad R.A.R., Wan Ahmad M.W. P., Mohamad N.M.S., Nagoor M. A., Vincent P. Assessing the big five personality on categories of athletes and gender among tennis players. Journal of Physical Education and Sport. 2022. Vol. 22. № 10. P. 2413–2419. DOI:10.7752/jpes.2022.10308.
  37. Sheppes G., Gross J.J. Is timing everything? Temporal considerations in emotion regulation. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 2011. Vol. 15, pp. 319–331. DOI:1177/1088868310395778
  38. Sheppes G., Gross J.J. Emotion Regulation Effectiveness: What Works When. Handbook of Psychology, 2012. Stanford, CA: John Wiley & Sons, Inc. pp. 391-403. DOI:10.1002/9781118133880.hop205018
  39. Tao Y, Liu X, Hou W, Niu H, Wang S, Ma Z, Bi D and Zhang L. The Mediating Role of Emotion Regulation Strategies in the Relationship Between Big Five Personality Traits and Anxiety and Depression Among Chinese Firefighters.  Public Health, 2022. 10:901686. DOI:10.3389/fpubh.2022.901686
  40. Uphill M. A., Lane A.M., Jones, M.V. Emotion Regulation Questionnaire for use with athletes. Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 2012. Vol. 13, no. 6, pp. 761-770.
  41. Wang L., Shi X., Li H. Neuroticism, extraversion, emotion regulation, negative affect and positive affect: the mediating roles of reappraisal and suppression. Social Behavior and Personality, 2009. Vol. 37, no. 2, pp. 193-194.
  42. Weinberg R.S., Gould D. Foundations of sport and exercise psychology. 1995. 1st ed. Champaign: Human Kinetics.
  43. Wilms R., Lanwehr R., and Kastenmüller A. Emotion regulation in everyday life: The role of goals and situational factors.  Frontiers in Psychology11 (2020): 877. DOI:10.3389/fpsyg.2020.00877
  44. Wilson K.E., Dishman R.K. Personality and physical activity: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Individ. Differ. 2015, Vol. 72, pp. 230–242.

Information About the Authors

Rita Karmakar, PhD in Psychology, Assistant Professor in Applied Psychology, Amity Institute of Psychology & Allied Sciences, Amity University, Kolkata, India, ORCID: https://orcid.org/0000-0002-0826-8816, e-mail: rk_r80@rediffmail.com

Aishee Ghosh, Research Student, Amity Institute of Psychology and Allied Sciences, Kolkata, India, ORCID: https://orcid.org/0000-0002-5358-3532, e-mail: aisheeghosh0101@gmail.com

Metrics

Views

Total: 553
Previous month: 22
Current month: 30

Downloads

Total: 277
Previous month: 7
Current month: 11