Psychological Science and Education
2022. Vol. 27, no. 4, 59–67
doi:10.17759/pse.2022270406
ISSN: 1814-2052 / 2311-7273 (online)
The Connection of Belief in a Just World with the Attitude to Academic Dishonesty among Schoolchildren with High and Low Loyalty to Cheating
Abstract
General Information
Keywords: belief in a Just world, belief in a just world for others, belief in Justice for oneself, academic dishonesty, academic honesty, cheating, students, education
Journal rubric: Educational Psychology
Article type: scientific article
DOI: https://doi.org/10.17759/pse.2022270406
Received: 23.03.2022
Accepted:
For citation: Ulybina E.V., Tokareva A.A. The Connection of Belief in a Just World with the Attitude to Academic Dishonesty among Schoolchildren with High and Low Loyalty to Cheating. Psikhologicheskaya nauka i obrazovanie = Psychological Science and Education, 2022. Vol. 27, no. 4, pp. 59–67. DOI: 10.17759/pse.2022270406.
Full text
Introduction
Academic dishonesty (cheating) is an unfortunate but fairly widespread occurance that includes a wide range of phenomena, from breaking rules to get better grades and other benefits to doing so to benefit others. In all cases, it have harmful consequences for both students and the educational system as a whole, particularly in reducing its credibility. In our study, academic dishonesty refers to any type of dishonesty associated with breaking the rules of academic activity.
Research on actual dishonesty shows that not all participants resort to dishonesty in an environment with a guaranteed lack of monitoring [10]. People seek both benefits and the preservation of a positive self-image. In the same conditions in which some schoolchildren cheat, others do not. A factor that may determine why is the notion of a just world structure.
According to M. Lerner [12], belief in a just world (BJW) is an adaptive illusion of the existence of rules, the observance of which will lead to success and violation of which will be punished.
The identification of two separate factors in the structure of belief in a just world — faith in a just world in general and faith in justice towards the subject [13] — showed that faith in justice towards the subject is directly connected with pro-social behavior [5; 21] because it corresponds to belief in the validity of rules [6], and faith in a just world in general — with a hostile attitude toward those who are in an unfavorable situation [20] because, if their suffering is not deserved, it prevents them from seeing the world as just.
In schoolchildren, the association of the belief in a just world with the self-assessment of cheating is mediated by the perceived justice of teachers [9; 17; 19]. This suggests that cheating and non-cheating students use different strategies in self-assessment.
Since belief in a just world reflects the idea that there are rules in the world and inevitable feedback on the observance thereof, it can be assumed that by high school age, adolescents are already forming an idea of the reality of formal and perceived so-called descriptive norms [7]. Descriptive norms may or may not coincide with prescriptive norms that contain information about how things should be. But if they do not coincide, as in the case of behavior that meets official disapproval, it is these norms that are more likely than prescriptive ones to predict, as meta-analysis of 196 individual studies shows, one’s actual behavior [15].
The assessment by a given student of the prevalence of cheating is one of the most useful predictors of academic dishonesty [16; 18], and for Russian students, perceived norms contribute more to one’s self-assessment of dishonesty than to one’s attitude toward dishonesty itself [14].
The prevailing norms are realized in the consequences of observance to and violation of them. For Russian students, cheating has no meaningful links with academic performance [2], but if knowledge is seen as important for later mastery and success, students are less likely to turn to dishonest behavior compared to those who pursue external goals [1; 4; 18]. At the same time, there is a fairly widespread belief in Russian culture that success in life is often achieved by low-performing students. Perhaps such attitudes legitimize dishonesty in the eyes of the students, allowing them to assess academic dishonesty as something that is characteristic of successful people and therefore, perhaps, a just and justifiable action.
Besides the intrinsic motivations above, the probability of subsequent punishment for cheating [1; 16; 18] can of course be a deterrent to dishonesty. It can be assumed in this case that, if one’s image of a just world is formed based on prescriptive norms, punishment for cheating can be seen as just, and if one’s image of justice is based on perceived norms, then, it can be assumed, punishment of cheating can be perceived as a violation of the de facto status quo, as an unjust action.
At the same time, longitudinal studies show that academic performance directly depends on belief in a just world, while belief in a just world does not depend on academic performance [8]. Those who believe in the possibility of achieving good grades through effort do achieve them. It is also possible that one’s assessment of how just one’s environmental , which mediates the connection between cheating and belief in a just world [8], is a product of academic effort among those students who believe that academic results will lead them to success in the future. Therefore, they do their homework, get good grades, and consider the existing rules to be just. Meanwhile, those who do not associate success with study may have an equal level of belief in a just world but do not consider academic requirements to be just.
The analysis conducted allowed the following hypotheses to be formulated:
Schoolchildren with faith in dishonesty and those without have different perceptions of the prevalence of academic dishonesty and its consequences. Those with such a faith believe cheating is more common, causes less trouble and less damage, and is particularly common among children who turn out to be successful than schoolchildren without such a faith do. Those with a faith in dishonesty consider academic knowledge less important for future success than non-cheaters, and agree to a greater extent that success is achieved by low-performing students, and that success cannot be achieved by honest means.
For students with high faith in dishonesty, faith in justice towards the subject has a direct relationship to the permissibility of dishonesty, the likelihood of success as a result of dishonesty, and inversely to punishment for dishonesty and possible harm to others.
When one’s faith in dishonesty is low, the belief in a just world includes official norms and the value of school knowledge. In this case, the belief in a just world is directly related to the likelihood of punishment for cheating, damage to others, and inversely to the perception that dishonesty at school age is peculiar to those who are successful and to the permissibility of dishonesty.
Methods and sampling
Sampling
A total of 516 students between the ages of 13 and 17, with an average age of 15.5, 274 of whom were female, and one who did not indicate gender, participated in the study.
Methods
Five vignettes were used to analyze attitudes toward academic dishonesty, describing different instances of cheating that included dishonesty for one’s own benefit and for the benefit of others:
1. Pupil A. used a cheat sheet on a test;
2. Pupil A. offered to write an essay for another pupil for money;
3. Pupil A. forged a note from his parents to avoid a test for which he was not ready;
4. Pupil A. wrote an essay for a classmate for money (other remuneration);
5. During a test, pupil A. passed the solution of the problems on his variant to the other pupils.
Judgments were offered for each variant of the situation, and agreement with them was assessed on a 10-point scale:
1. This is common behavior for students.
2. I believe that if it is absolutely necessary, it is okay to do this.
3. This action would hurt other people.
4. It would have unpleasant consequences for the student.
5. I assume that people who became successful used to do so as children as well.
Cronbach’s alpha for agreement with all judgments regarding the 5 vignettes was 0.778, indicating a fairly high consistency of responses and allowing for summary measurements of agreement with each statement to be used in the analysis of results.
The level of belief in a just world was measured using the scale of belief in a just world (C. Dalbert, Belief in a Just World) as adapted by S. K. Nartova-Bochaver and colleagues [3].
Statement of results
The data were processed using the Jamovi 2.2.5 statistical package.
Table 1 presents descriptive statistics of the sums of agreement scores for each of the 5 vignette statements.
The upper and lower quartiles of the agreement distribution with the statement that it is okay to cheat were used to identify sub-samples with high and low levels of permissibility.
Table 2 shows the sub-sample with low faith in dishonesty, which consisted of 142 respondents with scores from 1 to 25 (the lower quartile of the overall distribution) and for the sub-sample with high faith in dishonesty — 134 respondents with scores from 40 to 50.
Nonparametric criteria were used to process the results since an ordinal Likert scale was used to assess agreement with the dishonesty statements.
Spearman correlation analysis was used in Table 3 to test the hypothesis of a relationship between the belief in a just world scales and attitudes toward cheating.
Table 1
Statistics of age, scales of belief in a just world, and sums of the agreement scores for each of the statements on the 5 vignettes
Indicator |
Average |
Standard deviation |
Asymmetry (standard error 0.108) |
Excess (standard error 0.215) |
Age |
15.430 |
1.003 |
0.019 |
-0.894 |
Faith in justice towards the subject |
43.459 |
14.045 |
-0.083 |
-0.024 |
Faith in a just world in general* |
32.453 |
10.681 |
0.479 |
1.268 |
This is a common behavior for students |
29.736 |
9.390 |
0.085 |
-0.371 |
I believe that if it is absolutely necessary, it is okay to do this |
32.027 |
10.583 |
-0.098 |
-0.541 |
This action would hurt other people* |
18.647 |
9.790 |
0.674 |
0.044 |
There would have unpleasant consequences for the student |
27.465 |
9.639 |
-0.052 |
-0.425 |
I assume that people who became successful used to do so as children as well |
29.921 |
10.479 |
-0.065 |
-0.423 |
Note: * —distribution is significantly different from normal as per the Kolmogorov-Smirnov test.
Table 2
Descriptive statistics and comparison of age, attitude
toward justice,
and attitude toward dishonesty among students with low and high level
of faith in dishonesty using the Mann-Whitney test
Scales |
Low loyalty to dishonesty |
High loyalty to dishonesty |
U |
||
Average |
Standard Deviation |
Average |
Standard Deviation |
||
Age |
15.317 |
0.999 |
15.624 |
0.926 |
7790.5 |
Faith in justice towards the subject |
42.697 |
13.080 |
43.744 |
15.988 |
8859.0 |
Faith in a just world in general |
32.634 |
9.878 |
32.353 |
11.783 |
9162.0 |
This is a common behavior for students |
22.930 |
7.736 |
36.948 |
8.835 |
2286.5*** |
This action would hurt other people |
21.634 |
10.266 |
14.918 |
9.462 |
5690.5*** |
There would have unpleasant consequences for the student |
29.556 |
9.223 |
24.746 |
11.029 |
7020.5*** |
I assume that people who became successful used to do so as children as well |
21.958 |
7.634 |
37.052 |
11.186 |
2491.5*** |
Note: *-<0.05, **-<0.01, *** < 0.001, significance is given with Bonferroni correction.
Table 3
Relationship of the belief in a just world with attitudes toward cheating among students with high and low levels of faith in cheating
Scales |
Faith in justice towards the subject |
Faith in a just world in general |
||||
Sample total |
Among students with low faith in dishonesty |
Among students with high faith in dishonesty |
Sample total |
Among students with low faith in dishonesty |
Among students with high faith in dishonesty |
|
Age |
0.002 |
-0.133 |
0.080 |
-0.018 |
-0.048 |
0.069 |
This is a common behavior for students |
0.055 |
-0.135 |
0.224* |
0.082 |
0.027 |
0.270* |
I believe that if it is absolutely necessary, it is okay to do this |
0.046 |
-0.243* |
0.129 |
0.019 |
-0.075 |
0.114 |
This action would hurt other people |
-0.028 |
0.112 |
-0.088 |
0.069 |
0.100 |
0.045 |
There would have unpleasant consequences for the student |
0.111 |
0.234* |
0.048 |
0.130* |
0.135 |
0.145 |
I assume that people who became successful used to do so as children as well |
-0.016 |
-0.175 |
-0.049 |
-0.018 |
0.064 |
-0.023 |
Note: *-<0.05, **-<0.01, ***<0.001, significance is given with Bonferonni correction.
Discussion
The hypothesis of a difference in perceptions of the prevalence and consequences of dishonesty among schoolchildren with and without faith in it was confirmed. Ones with such a faith consider cheating to be more common, characteristic of successful people, entailing less trouble and leading to less damage than those without such a faith. The difference in the assessments of the prevalence and consequences of dishonesty in the same environment suggests the presence of filters that selectively capture and evaluate information that corresponds or does not correspond to one’s established image of reality.
The hypotheses about the difference in the structure of how one’s belief in a just world corresponds to one’s attitude toward dishonesty were partially confirmed. With low faith in dishonesty, faith in ‘justice towards the subject’ is inversely related to the permissibility of cheating and directly related to the likelihood of punishment. To the extent that what happens to them personally is assessed as a just result of their own actions, the negative consequences of dishonesty are also just.
For schoolchildren with high faith in dishonesty, the scales regarding a belief in a just world are related only to the assessment of the prevalence of cheating, but any correspondence with the possible consequences are insignificant. It can be assumed that either these students have not yet formed a stable idea of cause-and-effect relationships, or they are focused on norms and consequences not taken into account in the questions. The correlation of the belief in a just world with one’s assessment of the prevalence of dishonesty suggests that significant consequences also lie in the realm of actual peer relations, which needs to be verified.
Conclusions
The results confirm previous findings that there is no direct association of the belief in a just world with the permissibility of academic dishonesty among students.
However, students with high and low faith in cheating differ in the structure of their belief in a just world .
For students with low faith in dishonesty, the belief in a just world is inversely related to the permissibility of dishonesty and directly related to the probability of getting in trouble for such dishonesty. To the extent that the world is just to the subject himself, dishonesty is undesirable and punishment for it is likely.
For students who have faith in dishonesty, the scales of their belief in a just world are directly related only to their assessment of the prevalence of cheating; for them, the world is just to the extent that academic dishonesty is prevalent.
References
- Gizhitskii V.V. Uchebnyi obman kak strategiya psevdoadaptivnogo povedeniya u starsheklassnikov [Educational deception as a strategy of pseudo-adaptive behavior in high school students]. Uchenye zapiski Orlovskogo gosudarstvennogo universiteta. Seriya: Gumanitarnye i sotsial’nye nauki [Scientific notes of the Oryol State University. Series: Humanities and social sciences], 2014, no. 2(58), pp. 293—299. (In Russ.).
- Klimova K.K., Yachmeneva N.P. Svyaz’ urovnya akademicheskoi uspevaemosti so sklonnost’yu k akademicheskomu moshennichestvu [Relationship between academic performance and propensity for academic fraud]. Podrostok v megapolise: prodolzhennoe vzroslenie [A teenager in the metropolis: continued maturation], 2019, pp. 124—127. (In Russ.).
- Nartova-Bochaver S.K., Podlipnyak M.B., Khokhlova A.Yu. Vera v spravedlivyi mir i psikhologicheskoe blagopoluchie u glukhikh i slyshashchikh podrostkov i vzroslykh [Elektronnyi resurs] [Belief in a Just World and Psychological Well-Being in Deaf and Hearing Adolescents and Adults]. Klinicheskaya i spetsial’naya psikhologiya = Clinical and special psychology, 2013, no. 2(3). Available at: https://psyjournals.ru/files/64003/psyclin_2013_3_Nartova_Hohlova_Podlipnjak.pdf (Accessed 05.11.2021). (In Russ.).
- Anderman E., Griesinger T., Westerfield G. Motivation and cheating during early adolescence. Journal of Educational Psychology, 1998. Vol. 90(1), pp. 84—93. DOI:10.1037/0022-0663.90.1.84
- Bartholomaeus J., Strelan P. Just world beliefs and forgiveness: The mediating role of implicit theories of relationship. Personality and Individual Differences, 2016. Vol. 96, pp. 106—110. DOI:10.1016/j.paid.2016.02.081
- Bartholomaeus J., Strelan P. The adaptive, approach-oriented correlates of belief in a just world for the self: A review of the research. Personality and Individual Differences, 2019. Vol. 151, pp. 109—185. DOI:10.1016/j.paid.2019.06.028
- Cialdini R.B., Kallgren C.A., Reno R.R. A focus theory of normative conduct: Theoretical refinement and reevaluation of the role of norms in human behavior. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 1991. Vol. 24, pp. 201—243. DOI:10.1016/s0065-2601(08)60330-5
- Dalbert C., Stoeber J. The personal belief in a just world and domain-specific beliefs about justice at school and in the family: A longitudinal study with adolescents. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 2006. Vol. 30(3), pp. 200—207. DOI:10.1177/0165025406063638
- Donat M., Dalbert C., Kamble S.V. Adolescents’ cheating and delinquent behavior from a justice-psychological perspective: The role of teacher justice. European Journal of Psychology of Education, 2014. Vol. 29(4), pp. 635—651. DOI:10.1007/s10212-014-0218-5
- Fischbacher U., Föllmi-Heusi F. Lies in disguise-an experimental study on cheating. Journal of the European Economic Association, 2013. Vol. 11(3), pp. 525—547. DOI:10.1111/jeea.12014
- Furnham A. Belief in a just world: Research progress over the past decade. Personality and individual differences, 2003. Vol. 34(5), pp. 795—817. DOI:10.1016/S0191-8869(02)00072-7
- Lerner M.J. Integrating societal and psychological rules of entitlement: The basic task of each social actor and fundamental problem for the social sciences. Social Justice Research, 1987. Vol. 1(1), pp. 107—125. DOI:10.1007/bf01049386
- Lipkusa I.M., Dalbert C., Siegler I.C. The importance of distinguishing the belief in a just world for self versus for others: Implications for psychological well-being. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 1996. Vol. 22(7), pp. 666—677. DOI:10.1177/0146167296227002
- Maloshonok N., Shmeleva E. Factors influencing academic dishonesty among undergraduate students at Russian universities. Journal of Academic Ethics, 2019. Vol. 17(3), pp. 313—329. DOI:10.1007/s10805-019-9323-y
- Manning M. The effects of subjective norms on behaviour in the theory of planned behaviour: A meta-analysis. British journal of social psychology, 2019. Vol. 48(4), pp. 649—705. DOI:10.1348/014466608x393136
- McCabe D.L., Treviño L.K., Butterfield K.D. Cheating in academic institutions: A decade of research. Ethics & Behavior, 2001. Vol. 11(3), pp. 219—232. DOI:10.1207/s15327019eb1103_2
- Muenscher S., Donat M., Ucar G.K. Students’ Personal Belief in a Just World, Well-Being, and Academic Cheating: A Cross-National Study. Social Justice Research, 2020. Vol. 33(4), pp. 428—453. DOI:10.1007/s11211-020-00356-7
- Murdock T.B., Anderman E.M. Motivational perspectives on student cheating: Toward an integrated model of academic dishonesty. Educational psychologist, 2006. Vol. 41(3), pp. 129—145. DOI:10.1207/s15326985ep4103_1
- Sabbagh C. Self-reported academic performance and academic cheating: Exploring the role of the perceived classroom (in) justice mediators. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 2021. Vol. 91(4), pp. 1517—1536. DOI:10.1111/bjep.12433
- Sutton R.M., Douglas K.M. Justice for all, or just for me? More evidence of the importance of the self-other distinction in just-world beliefs. Personality and Individual Differences, 2005. Vol. 39(3), pp. 637—645. DOI:10.1016/j.paid.2005.02.010
- Sutton R.M., Stoeber J., Kamble S.V. Belief in a just world for oneself versus others, social goals, and subjective well-being. Personality and Individual Differences, 2017. Vol. 113, pp. 115—119. DOI:10.1016/j.paid.2017.03.026
Information About the Authors
Metrics
Views
Total: 701
Previous month: 17
Current month: 21
Downloads
Total: 247
Previous month: 5
Current month: 3