Clinical Psychology and Special Education
2022. Vol. 11, no. 1, 90–119
doi:10.17759/cpse.2022110105
ISSN: 2304-0394 (online)
Neurocognitive Models of Auditory Verbal Hallucinations in Schizophrenia: A Review.
Abstract
General Information
Keywords: auditory verbal hallucinations, schizophrenia, positive symptoms, neurocognitive models, neuropsychology, neuroimaging, neurophysiology
Journal rubric: Theoretical Research
Article type: scientific article
DOI: https://doi.org/10.17759/cpse.2022110105
Received: 02.09.2021
Accepted:
For citation: Panikratova Y.R., Lebedeva I.S. Neurocognitive Models of Auditory Verbal Hallucinations in Schizophrenia: A Review. [Elektronnyi resurs]. Klinicheskaia i spetsial'naia psikhologiia = Clinical Psychology and Special Education, 2022. Vol. 11, no. 1, pp. 90–119. DOI: 10.17759/cpse.2022110105.
Full text
Introduction
A recent trend in neurocognitive studies in psychiatry is characterized by a shift from the consideration of a mental disorder as a homogeneous nosological unit to the investigation of single clinical characteristics [26; 67]. Due to the heterogeneity of schizophrenia, separate analyses of its key clinical aspects may provide more information on the disorder than an attempt to comprehend an overall picture of its diverse characteristics.
Auditory verbal hallucinations (AVH) are a symptom common to various mental and neurological disorders (e.g., Parkinson’s disease, epilepsy, dementia, hearing impairments, bipolar disorder, and personality disorders) and are even found in nonclinical populations, but they are most prominent in the clinical picture of schizophrenia [37; 46]. AVH are one of the core positive symptoms of schizophrenia [26; 55] and an important criterion for the diagnosis of schizophrenia in the ICD-10. Kurt Schneider classified AVH as first-rank schizophrenia symptoms, including hearing thoughts spoken aloud, hearing two or more voices conversing with each other, and hearing voices commenting on a person's behavior [55]. This symptom is found in 70–80% patients with schizophrenia [41; 46; 67; 71] and causes pronounced distress [65]; AVH can be identified when a person in the awake state hears voices not triggered by a relevant external auditory stimulus but perceived to be realistic, uncontrollable, and different from the person’s own thoughts. An unequivocal and comprehensive determination of the psychological and neurophysiological mechanisms through which the production of a patient’s own mind is perceived as something alien is still lacking; a number of models have been suggested by different authors.
This review aims to analyze basic models of the neurocognitive mechanisms of AVH in patients with schizophrenia, including the results of relevant studies applying neuroimaging and neurophysiological techniques. As far as we know, the current article is the first literature review in Russian addressing the neurocognitive mechanisms of AVH. Such a review may encourage research interest in AVH within the Russian-speaking scientific community (specialists in clinical psychology, psychophysiology, and psychiatry) and facilitate the elaboration of existing hypotheses or the generation of new ones, whereas in practical terms it may help to improve protocols of neurocognitive rehabilitation.
Notably, the first models are described relatively briefly as they are the earliest and simplest and included in more complex models as component parts.
Models of intrusive cognitions and poor inhibitory control
A model of intrusive cognitions explains the emergence of AVH in schizophrenia patients through the presence of auditory representations which do not match current external stimuli, interfere with on-going cognitive processes and disrupt them [8]; they are associated with dysfunctionally high activity in the left temporal regions [39] involved in language comprehension. The model addresses intrusive thoughts, memories, and imagery [8; 16; 51]), including trauma-related intrusions [61]. Studies using questionnaires showed that intrusive thoughts were more frequently seen in schizophrenia patients with AVH, in contrast to patients without AVH and healthy individuals [51], and the severity of AVH was correlated to extra-list intrusions in memory tests [16; 17].
According to a model considered as an independent one by
some authors [8], schizophrenia patients with AVH demonstrate a deficit of an
inhibitory component of executive functions, namely the inhibition of the
above-mentioned intrusive cognitions
[9; 60; 69]. This may be related to the aberrant functioning of the
frontoparietal executive network [41]. Waters et al. [69], using the Hayling
Sentence Completion Test and Inhibition of Currently Irrelevant Memories task,
revealed that an inhibition deficit correlated to AVH frequency but was not
associated with other schizophrenia symptoms. Badcock et al. [9] also used the
latter task and showed that patients with AVH made more inhibition errors than
patients without AVH and healthy controls, which might point to a deficit of
selectivity of memory in this patient group. Soriano et al. [60] replicated and
extended these findings by applying the task measuring the ability to
intentionally forget recently learned information. In the study by Toh et al.
[64], only current voice-hearers, but not past or never voice-hearers with
schizophrenia, were characterized by inhibitory impairment. Taken together, the
first and the second models allow us to explain the sense of lacking voluntary
control over voices and their intrusive nature.
Model of an attentional shift to inner auditory stimuli
and an inability
to reallocate its resources, and a model of expectation maximization
The third group of models is based on the idea that
schizophrenia patients during AVH, partly due to attention switching
difficulties, spend most of their attentional resources (and expectations) on
listening to voices, which results in an aberrant processing of external
stimuli [67]. According to Friston’s Expectation-Maximization algorithm [29]
based on a Bayesian framework (for details see [42]), AVH may arise because of
the greater weight of prior expectations compared to sensory input, while the
level of uncertainty is underestimated. Consequently, the incoming prediction
error (i.e., the difference between the bottom-up signal and top-down
prediction) causes the patient to be unable to correct expectations, and a
false inference is made that the voices are real. Benrimoh et al. [12]
continued developing Friston’s ideas and focused on the activity of a subject
who may listen to voices, try to ignore them or answer them. Using
computational simulation, the authors found that the weight of prior beliefs
(i.e., the level of confidence in them) depends on beliefs about the
reliability of incoming sensory data as well as the monitoring of
a subject’s own actions (‘beliefs about policies’). Thus, the evaluation of
incoming sensory information as imprecise does not allow the person to correct
the false-positive hypothesis about the presence of voices; however, high
confidence that the person is listening at the moment is also necessary for the
emergence of AVH [12]. Importantly, the contribution of the subject’s activity
(i.e., listening) to the emergence of AVH was emphasized as early as 1970 in
the works by the Russian pathopsychologist Susanna Rubinshtein [5]. Horga and
Abi-Dargham [37] develop Friston’s ideas and suggest that an important
contribution to the reevaluation of prior expectations in AVH may be made by
aberrant functioning of the dopaminergic system and networks including the
striatum, which leads to a permanent sense of perceptual uncertainty and does
not allow a patient to dynamically adjust to its changing level; therefore, he
or she predominantly relies on prior expectations but not on incoming sensory
data.
Studies using the methodology of Signal Detection Theory (SDT) rely on the idea that perception always takes place under some uncertainty, and the detection of a stimulus depends both on perceptual sensitivity (i.e., an ability to detect a signal if it is really present) and perceptual bias. The experimental procedure implies a presentation of auditory stimuli masked by white noise and requires a participant to indicate the moment of voice detection. In such studies, patients with AVH demonstrate a false-positive response bias but not impaired perceptual sensitivity [14; 18; 24].
According to the results of neuroimaging studies, schizophrenia patients with AVH miss more target auditory stimuli and demonstrate less activation in the primary auditory cortex of the left hemisphere in response to a target stimulus compared to patients without AVH [28]. A meta-analysis [45] revealed that increased activation of the left primary auditory cortex was found in schizophrenia patients with AVH in the absence of external stimulation, while decreased activation was seen when they listened to speech. Dichotic listening studies identified the absence of a right-ear advantage in speech perception, which is common for healthy individuals, in patients with AVH [33; 40]. These results may seem to contradict the above data; in some cases, schizophrenia patients give increased attention to external stimuli (i.e., false-positive responses), while in other cases the patients miss them. The level of noise may be an important factor, as it additionally loads the auditory system and prompts patients to listen, which caused AVH in the studies by Rubinshtein and led to false-positive detections in SDT studies.
Model of working memory deficit
Working memory is one of the main components of executive functions that allows individuals to maintain and manipulate information that is necessary for the current activity, online. Working memory involves language subcomponents, namely rehearsal in the phonological loop. According to a number of studies, a working memory deficit plays an important role in the AVH mechanism [19; 32; 44; 63].
Bruder et al. [19] divided schizophrenia patients into two groups, those with and those without a core impairment in auditory information processing, based on tone discrimination test performance. The severity of AVH was associated with a verbal working memory deficit in patients with intact auditory information processing. The same patient group performed a verbal working memory task worse than both patients without AVH with intact auditory information processing and healthy individuals. Jenkins et al. [44], using hierarchical binary logistic regression, revealed that working memory (assessed with the MATRICS battery) predicted the presence of AVH in schizophrenia patients. Similar results were obtained in other studies [32; 63] regarding verbal working memory.
During working memory task performance, schizophrenia patients with AVH demonstrated decreased activation in the left temporoparietal regions involved in speech comprehension and verbal working memory compared to a clinical control group, and the decrease was negatively correlated to AVH severity [71].
Hoffman and McGlashan [36] developed a computer simulation
of working memory during recognition of a single word in a sentence. Two
conditions were used for the presentation of a word, normal and degraded
phonetic input, with the latter simulating
a reliance on working memory. When connections between different network layers
were disrupted, the system produced spontaneous percepts of words in the
absence of phonetic input and recognized words poorly under degraded phonetic
input. Schizophrenia patients with AVH made more word recognition errors
compared to control groups, which was best explained by an overpruned model
with changed activation of the network elements [36]. A detailed psychological
interpretation of the contribution of verbal working memory deficits to the AVH
mechanism is lacking; some authors [32; 71] suggest that AVH interfere with
external auditory stimuli, exploiting working memory resources (this hardly
differs from interpretations of the previous model).
Model of poor source-monitoring of speech
The next model prioritizes an impairment of
source-monitoring, a metacognitive function which allows an individual to
attribute his or her mental experience or actions, including language
production, to an external or internal source [68]. In typical tasks,
participants are asked to determine whether a particular mental action (e.g.,
utterance, movement) was performed by themselves or by someone else. For
instance, participants produce an association to each word in a series of
sequentially presented words and then identify the words generated by
themselves as well as the presented and non-presented words. A meta-analysis of
studies with different tasks [68] revealed a deficit of source-monitoring in
schizophrenia patients with AVH, in contrast to patients without AVH, which
took place at early stages of information processing (perception but not
memory). In
a study by Mechelli et al. [49], schizophrenia patients with AVH misidentified
their own speech as being that of somebody else more often than clinical and
nonclinical control groups. In healthy individuals and patients without AVH,
the effective connectivity of the left superior temporal gyrus with the
anterior cingulate gyrus was higher during listening to alien speech versus
self-generated speech, while patients with AVH demonstrated the reverse picture
[49]. Simons et al. [58] found that differences between listening to speech and
language production in the activation of left superior temporal gyrus were less
pronounced in schizophrenia patients with AVH than in controls. Therefore, the
perception of self-generated speech in schizophrenia patients with AVH may rely
on brain mechanisms underlying the perception of another person’s speech in
healthy individuals.
According to some authors, misattribution in patients with
AVH may be related to dysfunction of the right hemisphere [21; 59], which is
involved in online monitoring of incoming and transmitted information,
maintaining of the integrity of a mental model
of a situation as well as the correspondence of language and thinking to
reality, knowledge of the world, and life experience [1].
However, the reasons for source-monitoring deficits in AVH remain unclear in this model. In our opinion, they are highlighted in a model of poor verbal self-monitoring in inner speech.
Models of AVH within cultural-historical approach
A model of inner speech impairment in schizophrenia patients
with AVH by Charles Fernyhough [26] is rooted in Lev Vygotsky’s ideas of the
social genesis of higher mental functions as well as the dialogical nature,
abbreviation, and predicativity of inner speech. Fernyhough describes four
levels of inner speech development, namely external dialogue, private speech,
expanded inner speech, and condensed inner speech. Two possible mechanisms of
AVH are suggested: a disrupted internalization of inner speech
(i.e., a developmental impairment) and a compensatory re-expansion of inner
speech under conditions of stress and cognitive challenge. However, the
explanation of inner speech misattribution remains unclear in this model.
A model describing the psychological mechanisms of pathological alienation was proposed by Ignatiy Zhuravlev [2]. According to this model, AVH are perceived by a patient as resulting from the influence of an alien will because their main aspect, the sense of uncontrollability and being made by someone else, is also common to the perception of an external objective world. Pathological alienation, in the author’s opinion, is related to an impaired development of subjectivity. In normal conditions, subjectivity is organized through the distinction between a subject and an object, with the possibility of boundary shifting, but nevertheless, a stable range of its localization. If the boundary shifts outside this range, a thought may develop into AVH [2]. Subjectivity is considered a higher mental function; therefore, in its development, it acquires the possibility to be under voluntary control. This implies a polarization between an individual’s own and an alien possession, with a further interiorization of this distinction. Zhuravlev showed that schizophrenia patients in psychosis produced utterances not addressed to an interlocutor with increased frequency, with their further objectification as AVH often dialogical in content [2].
Model of poor verbal self-monitoring in inner speech
We assume that the model of poor verbal self-monitoring in inner speech [13; 25; 30] includes a majority of the previously described models as component parts and that it is the most integrative and elaborated model. According to this model, AVH in schizophrenia patients arise due to impaired self-monitoring in inner speech production and insufficient attenuation of its sensory consequences (disruption of corollary discharge). As the perception of inner speech is not coupled with signals indicating that an individual initiated this process himself or herself, a mental event (i.e., inner speech production) does not match its expected sensory consequences. Therefore, an individual is not ready to categorize his or her experience as inner speech and misattributes it to another source.
In terms of brain mechanisms, this deficit may rely on an increased activation of language areas due to aberrant functional connectivity with regions involved in executive functions [6]. Thus, increased bottom-up signals from the secondary auditory cortex involved in speech perception may be coupled with decreased top-down signals from the dorsolateral prefrontal, anterior cingulate, and supplementary motor cortices [6; 15]. Hugdahl [38] suggests additionally including the parietal regions contributing to an attentional shift to inner stimuli in this model. Waters et al. [67] underline an important role of negative emotions in the triggering and chronification of AVH in the model. Expectations, imagery, and memories individualizing the content of AVH, a lack of insight, delusional interpretations of AVH along with a false-positive perceptual bias and an inhibitory deficit are considered top-down processes [67]. The model is supported by data on the associations between the predominant language of AVH in bilinguals and an earlier age of language acquisition, more frequent language use, and subjectively higher language proficiency [34]. The involvement of an anticipatory corollary discharge mechanism in language processes in healthy individuals was demonstrated in several studies [56; 57]. Neurophysiological studies revealed that in healthy individuals but not in schizophrenia patients with AVH, amplitude characteristics of the N1 component of auditory event-related potentials reflected a dampening of the auditory cortex during language production [27]. The coherence of the theta rhythm between the frontal and temporal regions of the left hemisphere was higher in language production than in listening to speech in healthy individuals but not in patients with AVH [27]. Further studies found a delay in auditory cortex suppression, associated with a decreased fractional anisotropy of the arcuate fasciculus, in schizophrenia patients with AVH [70].
As inner speech underlies executive functions (according to the ideas originating from Lev Vygotsky and supported by many contemporary authors; see paragraphs 1.2 and 1.3 in [4]), its impairments may lead to poor performance in a range of tasks loading different components of executive functions, including inhibition, switching, and working memory [53], in schizophrenia patients with AVH. These components were mentioned in previous models. At the same time, an executive deficit per se may contribute to inner speech impairments [53].
In the first group of fMRI studies relevant to this model,
AVH are considered to be associated with a stable aberration of functional
brain architecture (trait studies), and schizophrenia patients with AVH are
compared to clinical and nonclinical control groups. During imagining sentences
versus listening to them, healthy individuals had decreased activation of the
left superior temporal gyrus, and this decrease was less pronounced in
schizophrenia patients with AVH [58]. Activation of the cingulate gyrus in
healthy participants was higher in imagining sentences than listening to them,
while patients demonstrated the reverse trend [58]. The severity of AVH in
schizophrenia patients was associated with decreased functional connectivity
between the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and left temporal regions in
sentence completion [47]. Studies using resting-state fMRI also revealed
aberrant functional connectivity between the anterior cingulate cortex and left
temporal regions in patients with AVH [20; 66]. At the same time, according to
our study [4], which included schizophrenia patients with a history of AVH,
patients without
a history of AVH, and healthy individuals, decreased functional connectivity
between the anterior cingulate cortex and the superior temporal gyrus
bilaterally was not a specific trait of patients with AVH but was a common
characteristic of all schizophrenia patients.
A specific trait of patients with AVH was decreased functional connectivity
between the left inferior frontal gyrus, which is involved in language
production, and the anterior cingulate cortex.
The second group of fMRI studies is based on the idea that
AVH are related to
a temporary change in brain functioning (state studies), including the
increased activation of a network involved in language production and
perception [7; 15; 31; 43]. The subjective reality of voices in patients with
schizophrenia spectrum disorders is correlated with the functional connectivity
of the inferior frontal gyrus with temporal regions and the anterior cingulate
cortex [54]. According to some authors, brain activation during AVH is not
lateralized to any hemisphere [7], while other data suggest that the symptom is
related to predominant right hemisphere activation [15; 23; 59]. Sommer and
Diederen [59] propose that the key mechanism of AVH is the insufficient
inhibition of right-hemisphere language areas by the anterior cingulate cortex.
The authors suggest that negative content, intrusive aspect, and a lack of
voluntary control are similar for AVH in schizophrenia and “automatic speech”
in aphasia due to left hemisphere injury and right hemisphere disinhibition. An
additional argument provided by the authors concerns the data indicating that
language processes in healthy individuals rely on the inhibition of
right-hemisphere homologues of language areas [59]. At the same time, a recent
meta-analysis by Barber et al. [11] did not replicate the activation of
inferior frontal and superior temporal gyri in any hemisphere during AVH in
schizophrenia patients (perhaps due to an application of conservative
statistical thresholds); however, they revealed a cluster of activation in the
left insula which, on the one hand, is involved in language, and on the other
hand, is a component of the salience network.
Data on therapy of AVH with noninvasive brain stimulation also support this model of AVH [10; 48; 50]. For instance, transcranial direct current stimulation with the cathode (inhibitory effect) placed over the left temporoparietal junction and the anode (excitatory effect) placed over the left prefrontal cortex reduced treatment-resistant AVH in schizophrenia patients [50].
Conclusions
We have considered the main models of the neurocognitive
mechanisms of AVH in patients with schizophrenia. Although the reviewed models
focus on different aspects of AVH, the models overlap and complement each
other. The first model prioritizes the intrusive cognitions (thoughts,
memories, or imagery) interfering with on-going cognitive processes and
disrupting them. The next models address the reasons for the inability to avoid
these intrusions, namely a deficit of executive functions such as inhibition,
switching, and working memory, which explains the sense of lacking voluntary
control over the voices heard by a patient. Specifically, a group of models
emphasizes an attentional shift to inner stimuli and an inability to switch to
external stimuli. Thus, according to a Bayesian model of expectation
maximization, AVH may arise due to the dominance of prior expectations over
novel incoming information and an underestimation of the uncertainty
level.
A number of authors [5; 12] underline the importance of an individual’s
activity
(i.e., listening) in the psychological mechanism of AVH. Another significant
model is based on the idea of poor source-monitoring of the mental experience,
in particular language production, i.e., its misattribution to external or
internal sources. This misattribution error is explained by the dialogical
nature of inner speech and its development through interiorization in the
models within a cultural-historical approach. Finally, in our opinion, the most
integrative and elaborated model is that of poor verbal self-monitoring in
inner speech. According to this model, AVH may arise due to an impaired
anticipation of the sensory consequences of inner speech; since such prediction
is absent, a patient is unable to categorize the ongoing experience as his or
her own inner speech and misattributes it to another source. Methodological
limitations of existing fMRI studies of the brain mechanisms of AVH in
schizophrenia patients are briefly discussed elsewhere [3].
Based on our review of neurocognitive models of AVH in schizophrenia patients, we can conclude that this clinical group demonstrates deficits in executive functions and language, or rather a deficit in the cross-functional interaction between them that is particularly evident in the last model. This conclusion coincides with the data suggesting that deficits in executive functions [52; 62] and language [21; 22; 35; 72] are the key cognitive disturbances in schizophrenia patients. In this context, it is possible that schizophrenia patients with AVH have specific traits which differentiate them from other schizophrenia patients, along with neuropsychological impairments somehow common for all patients with schizophrenia.
Limitations of the current literature review are associated
with the fact that it is not
a systematic review. These limitations are due to a large number of
publications that cannot be analyzed within the scope of one article as well as
difficulties in the formulation of precise criteria for the literature search
within the defined issue. Nevertheless, it is possible to conduct a systematic
review of articles within each of the described models.
References
-
Akhutina T.V. Rol' pravogo
polushariya v postroenii teksta [Role of the right hemisphere in text
construction].
Psikholіngvіstika=Psycholinguistics,
2009, no. 3,
pp. 10–28. (In Russ.) - Zhuravlev I.V. Semiotiko-psikhologicheskie mekhanizmy otchuzhdeniya pri sindrome psikhicheskogo avtomatizma. Diss. kand. psikhol. nauk. [Semiotic and psychological mechanisms of alienation in patients with the syndrome of mental automatism. PhD (Psychology) diss.]. Moscow, 2003. 177 p. (In Russ.)
- Panikratova Ya.R. Analysis of brain mechanisms of auditory verbal hallucinations in schizophrenia patients via fMRI. Vestnik Moskovskogo universiteta. Seriya 7: Filosofiya=Moscow University Bulletin. Series 7: Philosophy, 2021, no. 6, pp. 18–26. (In Russ., abstr. in Engl.)
- Panikratova Ya.R. Osobennosti vzaimodeistviya rechevykh i regulyatornykh funktsii u bol'nykh s verbal'nymi gallyutsinatsiyami. Diss. kand. psikhol. nauk. [Interaction between language and executive functions in patients with auditory verbal hallucinations. PhD (Psychology) diss.]. Moscow, 2021. 126 p. (In Russ.)
- Rubinshtein S.Ya. Eksperimental'noe issledovanie obmanov slukha [Experimental study of auditory hallucinations]. Voprosy Patopsikhologii=Issues of Pathopsychology. Moscow, 1970, pp. 55–69. (In Russ.)
- Allen P., Laroi F., McGuire P.K. et al. The hallucinating brain: a review of structural and functional neuroimaging studies of hallucinations. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 2008, vol. 32, no. 1, pp. 175–191. DOI: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2007.07.012
- Allen P., Modinos G., Hubl D. et al. Neuroimaging auditory hallucinations in schizophrenia: from neuroanatomy to neurochemistry and beyond. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 2012, vol. 38, no. 4, pp. 695–703. DOI: 10.1093/schbul/sbs066
- Badcock J.C., Hugdahl K. Cognitive mechanisms of auditory verbal hallucinations in psychotic and non-psychotic groups. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 2012, vol. 36, no. 1, pp. 431–438. DOI: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2011.07.010
- Badcock J.C., Waters F.A., Maybery M.T. et al. Auditory hallucinations: failure to inhibit irrelevant memories. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, 2005, vol. 10, no. 2, pp. 125–136. DOI: 10.1080/13546800344000363
-
Bais L., Liemburg E., Vercammen A. et al. Effects of low
frequency rTMS treatment on brain networks for inner speech in patients with
schizophrenia and auditory verbal hallucinations. Progress in
Neuro-Psychopharmacology & Biological Psychiatry, 2017,
vol. 78, pp. 105–113. DOI: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2017.04.017 - Barber L., Reniers R., Upthegrove R. A review of functional and structural neuroimaging studies to investigate the inner speech model of auditory verbal hallucinations in schizophrenia. Translational Psychiatry, 2021, vol. 11, no. 1, P. 582. DOI: 10.1038/s41398-021-01670-7
- Benrimoh D., Parr T., Vincent P. et al. Active inference and auditory hallucinations. Computational Psychiatry, 2018, vol. 2, pp. 183–204. DOI: 10.1162/cpsy_a_00022
- Bentall R.P. The illusion of reality: a review and integration of psychological research on hallucinations. Psychological Bulletin, 1990, vol. 107, no. 1, pp. 82–95. DOI: 10.1037/0033-2909.107.1.82
-
Bentall R.P., Slade P.D. Reality testing and auditory
hallucinations: a signal detection analysis. The British Journal of Clinical
Psychology, 1985, vol. 24 (Pt 3),
pp. 159–169. DOI: 10.1111/j.2044-8260.1985.tb01331.x - Bohlken M.M., Hugdahl K., Sommer I.E. Auditory verbal hallucinations: neuroimaging and treatment. Psychological Medicine, 2017, vol. 47, no. 2, pp. 199–208. DOI: 10.1017/S003329171600115X
- Brebion G., David A.S., Bressan R.A. et al. Hallucinations and two types of free-recall intrusion in schizophrenia. Psychological Medicine, 2009, vol. 39, no. 6, pp. 917–926. DOI: 10.1017/S0033291708004819
- Brebion G., Stephan-Otto C., Ochoa S. et al. Impaired self-monitoring of inner speech in schizophrenia patients with verbal hallucinations and in non-clinical individuals prone to hallucinations. Frontiers in Psychology, 2016, vol. 7, p. 1381. DOI: 10.3389/ fpsyg.2016.01381
- Brookwell M.L., Bentall R.P., Varese F. Externalizing biases and hallucinations in source-monitoring, self-monitoring and signal detection studies: a meta-analytic review. Psychological Medicine, 2013, vol. 43, no. 12, pp. 2465–2475. DOI: 10.1017/S0033291 712002760
- Bruder G.E., Alschuler D.M., Kroppmann C.J. et al. Heterogeneity of auditory verbal working memory in schizophrenia. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 2011, vol. 120, no. 1, pp. 88–97. DOI: 10.1037/a0021661
-
Chang X., Collin G., Xi Y. et al. Resting-state functional
connectivity in medication-naive schizophrenia patients with and without
auditory verbal hallucinations:
A preliminary report. Schizophrenia Research, 2017, vol. 188, pp. 75–81. DOI: 10.1016/ j.schres.2017.01.024 - Crow T.J. Is schizophrenia the price that Homo sapiens pays for language? Schizophrenia Research, 1997, vol. 28, no. 2–3, pp. 127–141. DOI: 10.1016/s0920-9964(97)00110-2
- de Boer J.N., Brederoo S.G., Voppel A.E. et al. Anomalies in language as a biomarker for schizophrenia. Current Opinion in Psychiatry, 2020, vol. 33, no. 3, pp. 212–218. DOI: 10.1097/YCO.0000000000000595
- Diederen K.M., De Weijer A.D., Daalman K. et al. Decreased language lateralization is characteristic of psychosis, not auditory hallucinations. Brain: a Journal of Neurology, 2010, vol. 133, no. Pt 12, pp. 3734–3744. DOI: 10.1093/brain/awq313
-
Dodgson G., Gordon S. Avoiding false negatives: are some
auditory hallucinations an evolved design flaw? Behavioural and Cognitive
Psychotherapy, 2009, vol. 37, no. 3,
pp. 325–334. DOI: 10.1017/S1352465809005244 - Feinberg I. Efference copy and corollary discharge: implications for thinking and its disorders. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 1978, vol. 4, no. 4, pp. 636–640. DOI: 10.1093/schbul/ 4.4.636
- Fernyhough C. Alien voices and inner dialogue: towards a developmental account of auditory verbal hallucinations. New Ideas in Psychology, 2004, vol. 22, no. 1, pp. 49–68. DOI: 10.1016/j.newideapsych.2004.09.001
- Ford J.M., Mathalon D.H. Electrophysiological evidence of corollary discharge dysfunction in schizophrenia during talking and thinking. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 2004, vol. 38, no. 1, pp. 37–46. DOI: 10.1016/s0022-3956(03)00095-5
- Ford J.M., Roach B.J., Jorgensen K.W. et al. Tuning in to the voices: a multisite FMRI study of auditory hallucinations. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 2009, vol. 35, no. 1, pp. 58–66. DOI: 10.1093/schbul/sbn140
- Friston K. The free-energy principle: a unified brain theory? Nature Reviews. Neuroscience, 2010, vol. 11, no. 2, pp. 127–138. DOI: 10.1038/nrn2787
- Frith C. The cognitive neuropsychology of schizophrenia. Hove, UK: Psychology Press, 1992. 169 p.
- Fuentes-Claramonte P., Soler-Vidal J., Salgado-Pineda P. et al. Auditory hallucinations activate language and verbal short-term memory, but not auditory, brain regions. Scientific Reports, 2021, vol. 11, no. 1, P. 18890. DOI: 10.1038/s41598-021-98269-1
- Gisselgard J., Anda L.G., Bronnick K. et al. Verbal working memory deficits predict levels of auditory hallucination in first-episode psychosis. Schizophrenia Research, 2014, vol. 153, no. 1–3, pp. 38–41. DOI: 10.1016/j.schres.2013.12.018
-
Green M.F., Hugdahl K., Mitchell S. Dichotic listening
during auditory hallucinations in patients with schizophrenia. The American
Journal of Psychiatry, 1994, vol. 151, no. 3,
pp. 357–362. DOI: 10.1176/ajp.151.3.357 -
Hadden L.M., Alderson-Day B., Jackson M. et al. The
auditory-verbal hallucinations of Welsh-English bilingual people. Psychology
and Psychotherapy, 2020, vol. 93, no. 1,
pp. 122–133. DOI: 10.1111/papt.12234 - Hinzen W., Rossello J. The linguistics of schizophrenia: thought disturbance as language pathology across positive symptoms. Frontiers in Psychology, 2015, vol. 6, P. 971. DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00971
- Hoffman R.E., McGlashan T.H. Using a speech perception neural network computer simulation to contrast neuroanatomic versus neuromodulatory models of auditory hallucinations. Pharmacopsychiatry, 2006, vol. 39, suppl. 1, pp. S54–S64. DOI: 10.1055/s-2006-931496
- Horga G., Abi-Dargham A. An integrative framework for perceptual disturbances in psychosis. Nature Reviews. Neuroscience, 2019, vol. 20, no. 12, pp. 763–778. DOI: 10.1038/s41583-019-0234-1
- Hugdahl K. "Hearing voices": auditory hallucinations as failure of top-down control of bottom-up perceptual processes. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 2009, vol. 50, no. 6, pp. 553–560. DOI: 10.1111/j.1467-9450.2009.00775.x
- Hugdahl K. Auditory Hallucinations as Translational Psychiatry: Evidence from Magnetic Resonance Imaging. Balkan Medical Journal, 2017, vol. 34, no. 6, pp. 504–513. DOI: 10.4274/balkanmedj.2017.1226
- Hugdahl K., Loberg E.M., Jorgensen H.A. et al. Left hemisphere lateralisation of auditory hallucinations in schizophrenia: a dichotic listening study. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, 2008, vol. 13, no. 2, pp. 166–179. DOI: 10.1080/13546800801906808
- Hugdahl K., Loberg E.M., Nygard M. Left temporal lobe structural and functional abnormality underlying auditory hallucinations in schizophrenia. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 2009, vol. 3, no. 1, pp. 34–45. DOI: 10.3389/neuro.01.001.2009
- Jardri R., Denève S. Computational Models of Hallucinations. In Jardri R. et al. (eds.) The Neuroscience of Hallucinations. New-York: Springer, 2013, pp. 289–313.
- Jardri R., Pouchet A., Pins D. et al. Cortical activations during auditory verbal hallucinations in schizophrenia: a coordinate-based meta-analysis. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 2011, vol. 168, no. 1, pp. 73–81. DOI: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2010.09101522
- Jenkins L.M., Bodapati A.S., Sharma R.P. et al. Working memory predicts presence of auditory verbal hallucinations in schizophrenia and bipolar disorder with psychosis. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 2018, vol. 40, no. 1, pp. 84–94. DOI: 10.1080/13803395.2017.1321106
- Kompus K., Westerhausen R., Hugdahl K. The "paradoxical" engagement of the primary auditory cortex in patients with auditory verbal hallucinations: a meta-analysis of functional neuroimaging studies. Neuropsychologia, 2011, vol. 49, no. 12, pp. 3361–3369. DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.08.010
- Laroi F., Sommer I.E., Blom J.D. et al. The characteristic features of auditory verbal hallucinations in clinical and nonclinical groups: state-of-the-art overview and future directions. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 2012, vol. 38, no. 4, pp. 724–733. DOI: 10.1093/schbul/ sbs061
- Lawrie S.M., Buechel C., Whalley H.C. et al. Reduced frontotemporal functional connectivity in schizophrenia associated with auditory hallucinations. Biological Psychiatry, 2002, vol. 51, no. 12, pp. 1008–1011. DOI: 10.1016/s0006-3223(02)01316-1
-
Li J., Cao X., Liu S. et al. Efficacy of repetitive
transcranial magnetic stimulation on auditory hallucinations in schizophrenia:
A meta-analysis. Psychiatry Research, 2020,
vol. 290, P. 113141. DOI: 10.1016/j.psychres.2020.113141 - Mechelli A., Allen P., Amaro E.Jr. et al. Misattribution of speech and impaired connectivity in patients with auditory verbal hallucinations. Human Brain Mapping, 2007, vol. 28, no. 11, pp. 1213–1222. DOI: 10.1002/hbm.20341
-
Mondino M., Haesebaert F., Poulet E. et al.
Fronto-temporal transcranial Direct Current Stimulation (tDCS) reduces
source-monitoring deficits and auditory hallucinations in patients with
schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research, 2015, vol. 161, no. 2–3,
pp. 515–516. DOI: 10.1016/j.schres.2014.10.054 -
Morrison A.P., Baker C.A. Intrusive thoughts and auditory
hallucinations:
a comparative study of intrusions in psychosis. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 2000,
vol. 38, no. 11, pp. 1097–1106. DOI: 10.1016/s0005-7967(99)00143-6 - Orellana G., Slachevsky A. Executive functioning in schizophrenia. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 2013, vol. 4, P. 35. DOI: 10.3389/fpsyt.2013.00035
- Petrolini V., Jorba M., Vicente A. The role of inner speech in executive functioning tasks: schizophrenia with auditory verbal hallucinations and autistic spectrum conditions as case studies. Frontiers in Psychology, 2020, vol. 11, P. 572035. DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg. 2020.572035
- Raij T.T., Valkonen-Korhonen M., Holi M. et al. Reality of auditory verbal hallucinations. Brain: a Journal of Neurology, 2009, vol. 132, no. Pt 11, pp. 2994–3001. DOI: 10.1093/brain/awp186
- Schneider K. Clinical psychopathology. New York: Grune & Stratton, 1959. 173 p.
- Scott M. Corollary discharge provides the sensory content of inner speech. Psychological Science, 2013, vol. 24, no. 9, pp. 1824–1830. DOI: 10.1177/09567976 13478614
- Scott M., Yeung H.H., Gick B. et al. Inner speech captures the perception of external speech. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 2013, vol. 133, no. 4, EL286–292. DOI: 10.1121/1.4794932
- Simons C.J., Tracy D.K., Sanghera K.K. et al. Functional magnetic resonance imaging of inner speech in schizophrenia. Biological Psychiatry, 2010, vol. 67, no. 3, pp. 232–237. DOI: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.09.007
- Sommer I.E., Diederen K.M. Language production in the non-dominant hemisphere as a potential source of auditory verbal hallucinations. Brain: A Journal of Neurology, 2009, vol. 132, no. 10, pp. e124–e124. DOI: 10.1093/brain/awp040
- Soriano M.F., Jimenez J.F., Roman P. et al. Inhibitory processes in memory are impaired in schizophrenia: evidence from retrieval induced forgetting. British Journal of Psychology, 2009, vol. 100, Pt 4, pp. 661–673. DOI: 10.1348/000712609X418912
- Steel C., Fowler D., Holmes E.A. Trauma-Related Intrusions and Psychosis: An Information Processing Account. Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapy, 2004, vol. 33, no. 2, pp. 139–152. DOI: 10.1017/s1352465804001924
- Thai M.L., Andreassen A.K., Bliksted V. A meta-analysis of executive dysfunction in patients with schizophrenia: Different degree of impairment in the ecological subdomains of the Behavioural Assessment of the Dysexecutive Syndrome. Psychiatry Research, 2019, vol. 272, pp. 230–236. DOI: 10.1016/j.psychres.2018.12.088
- Thoma R.J., Haghani Tehrani P., Turner J.A. et al. Neuropsychological analysis of auditory verbal hallucinations. Schizophrenia Research, 2018, vol. 192, pp. 459–460. DOI: 10.1016/j.schres.2017.03.044
- Toh W.L., Tan E.J., Neill E. et al. Identifying the cognitive underpinnings of voice-hearing by comparing never, past and current voice-hearers. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 2020, vol. 141, no. 6, pp. 553–562. DOI: 10.1111/acps.13169
- Tsang A., Bucci S., Branitsky A. et al. The relationship between appraisals of voices (auditory verbal hallucinations) and distress in voice-hearers with schizophrenia-spectrum diagnoses: A meta-analytic review. Schizophrenia Research, 2021, vol. 230, pp. 38–47. DOI: 10.1016/j.schres.2021.02.013
- Vercammen A., Knegtering H., den Boer J.A. et al. Auditory hallucinations in schizophrenia are associated with reduced functional connectivity of the temporo-parietal area. Biological Psychiatry, 2010, vol. 67, no. 10, pp. 912–918. DOI: 10.1016/j.biopsych. 2009.11.017
- Waters F., Allen P., Aleman A. et al. Auditory hallucinations in schizophrenia and nonschizophrenia populations: a review and integrated model of cognitive mechanisms. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 2012, vol. 38, no. 4, pp. 683–693. DOI: 10.1093/schbul/sbs045
- Waters F., Woodward T., Allen P. et al. Self-recognition deficits in schizophrenia patients with auditory hallucinations: a meta-analysis of the literature. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 2012, vol. 38, no. 4, pp. 741–750. DOI: 10.1093/schbul/sbq144
-
Waters F.A.V., Badcock J.C., Maybery M.T. et al.
Inhibition in schizophrenia: association with auditory hallucinations.
Schizophrenia Research, 2003, vol. 62, no. 3,
pp. 275–280. DOI: 10.1016/s0920-9964(02)00358-4 - Whitford T.J., Mathalon D.H., Shenton M.E. et al. Electrophysiological and diffusion tensor imaging evidence of delayed corollary discharges in patients with schizophrenia. Psychological Medicine, 2011, vol. 41, no. 5, pp. 959–969. DOI: 10.1017/S00332917 10001376
- Wible C.G., Lee K., Molina I. et al. fMRI activity correlated with auditory hallucinations during performance of a working memory task: data from the FBIRN consortium study. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 2009, vol. 35, no. 1, pp. 47–57. DOI: 10.1093/ schbul/sbn142
-
Zimmerer V.C., Watson S., Turkington D. et al. Deictic and
Propositional Meaning-New Perspectives on Language in Schizophrenia.
Frontiers in Psychiatry, 2017, vol. 8,
pp. 17. DOI: 10.3389/fpsyt.2017.00017
Information About the Authors
Metrics
Views
Total: 783
Previous month: 20
Current month: 18
Downloads
Total: 365
Previous month: 5
Current month: 6