Clinical Psychology and Special Education
2020. Vol. 9, no. 3, 15–33
doi:10.17759/cpse.2020090302
ISSN: 2304-0394 (online)
A Dual-System Model of Social Anxiety Disorder: The Interplay of the Social-Rank and Affiliation Biopsychosocial Systems
Abstract
General Information
Keywords: dominance, belongingness, social anxiety, exclusion, defeat, depression, social stress
Journal rubric: Theoretical Research
Article type: scientific article
DOI: https://doi.org/10.17759/cpse.2020090302
For citation: Gilboa-Schechtman E. A Dual-System Model of Social Anxiety Disorder: The Interplay of the Social-Rank and Affiliation Biopsychosocial Systems [Elektronnyi resurs]. Klinicheskaia i spetsial'naia psikhologiia = Clinical Psychology and Special Education, 2020. Vol. 9, no. 3, pp. 15–33. DOI: 10.17759/cpse.2020090302.
Full text
A Dual-System Model of Social Anxiety Disorder: The Interplay of the Social-Rank and Affiliation Biopsychosocial Systems
Social dangers constitute the main perils of modern life. Acceptance is never a given, prestige is never secure. Every social encounter involves a risk of rejection and a possibility of humiliation. Whereas a certain level of anxiety in the face of exclusion and defeat is adaptive, intense anxiety can significantly impair coping with such events. Social Anxiety Disorder (SAD) is a condition involving marked anxiety about social or performance situations in which an individual is exposed to possible scrutiny by others with fear of public speaking being the most common concern [70]. Individuals with SAD fear acting in ways that will be humiliating, embarrassing, or will lead to rejection [6].
The Clinical Profile of Social Anxiety Disorder (SAD)
Life-time prevalence of SAD is estimated around 12% and is associated with severe psychological, interpersonal, and professional consequences resulting in quality of life impairment [3; 78]. Social anxiety (SA) ranges in severity, but even below-diagnostic levels are associated with reduced well-being [30], and lower quality of intimacy in peer, friend, and romantic relationships [106].
The clinical profile of SAD is marked by an early onset with almost 75% of the cases beginning by mid-adolescence [43]. Women are more likely to have SAD [10]. SAD is comorbid with major depressive disorder (MDD, [67]) and with substance abuse disorders [16]. Significantly, the onset of SAD precedes the onsets of both MDD and substance abuse disorders in as many as 80% of comorbid cases [83]. It is therefore unsurprising that individuals with SAD are likely to attempt suicide [47].
Multiple intervention types have shown promise in the treatment of SAD including (but not limited to) Cognitive Behavioral Therapy (CBT), acceptance and commitment therapy, interpersonal psychotherapy, and social skills training [2]. Multiple outcome studies examined the effectiveness of individual as well as group formats of CBT, documenting moderate to large effect sizes [65]. However, despite the effectiveness of CBT, many patients either do not stay in therapy (attrition rates of 5-30% [1; 51]), fail to respond to CBT (40-57% do not exhibit clinically significant symptom reduction even after completing the full course, [20]) or remain considerably symptomatic at the end of treatment (only about 1 in 5 individuals reached symptom-free functioning in a large scale community study of group CBT [9]). Moreover, even following the completion of a full course of CBT, many patients continue to report reduced well-being and satisfaction with the quality of their interpersonal relationships [20; 27].
Theoretical Accounts of SAD: Process and System Models
Cognitive models emphasize the ways in which various processes combine to maintain SAD. According to these models, high-SA individuals are driven by a desire to make a good impression, yet doubt their ability to achieve this goal [18; 45; 48; 66; 72]. SAD is also postulated to be associated with enhanced self‐focus to internal sensations, resulting in painful self‐awareness. Moreover, it is believed that SAD is perpetuated by negative cognitions and images of the self. Rapee and Heimberg also postulate that biased attention to threatening interpersonal cues contributes to the maintenance of SAD [72]. Hofmann further argues that individuals with SAD are characterized by enhanced discrepancies between actual and ideal self, and that these discrepancies may underlie their fears about their ability to maintain a desired image in the eyes of others [48]. Moscovitch stresses the importance of identifying specific self‐attributes that underlie individuals’ negative self‐concept, rather than focusing on the generally negative contents of their self‐beliefs [66]. These theories highlight the role of multiple cognitive processes – such as attention, interpretation, and evaluation – in the maintenance of SAD.
Another set of models examines SAD from the perspective of basic biobehavioral systems, highlighting the possible ways in which such systems may malfunction to produce the clinical profile of SAD. Two such prominent systems are the affiliation (aka belongingness) and social-rank (aka power, dominance) systems. Some models postulate that SAD is only associated with one of the two, in particular, the social-rank system [46; 105]. Hermans and van Honk propose that the adaptive function of SA is rooted in ancient communicative systems that regulate social order and inhibit inappropriate and antisocial behaviors [46]. Weeks and colleagues elaborate this line of thinking, suggesting that the tendency to avoid evaluations and to exhibit submissive behaviors may be helpful in coping with social threats by dodging conflicts with powerful others [105].
Other models argue that SA needs to be considered from the perspectives of both systems [36; 99]. Trower and Gilbert propose that SA individuals tend to over-utilize the social-rank and under-utilize the affiliation systems [99]. They further argue that SA individuals are attuned to cues of dominance often at the expense of signals of affiliation. Further elaborating the dual-system account, Gilboa-Schechtman and colleagues emphasize the combined role of the social-rank and affiliation systems in the etiology and maintenance of SAD [4; 34; 35; 42]. They propose that while social cautiousness may be advantageous in unstable hierarchies, it may backfire in moderately benevolent and cohesive social groups.
Purpose and Structure of the Review
Despite the major advantages achieved in the understanding
of SAD under the theoretical guidance of both the process and the
system-models, gaps in existing conceptualization and knowledge loom large.
First, a robust literature documents the deficits in affiliative
relationships associated with SAD [5]. Yet, many state-of-the-art approaches,
such as CBT, do not examine the outcome of treatment in terms of reduction of
these deficits. The restrictive focus on social avoidance and submissiveness
features of SAD in CBT may contribute to the partial success of these
treatments. The importance of addressing multiple interpersonal deficits in SAD
is underscored by studies highlighting the partial independence of affiliative
and social-rank biobehavioral systems [17; 44; 61]. Second, existing
perspectives do not offer an integrative framework for the broad clinical
picture of SAD – including developmental trajectory, gender differences, and
patterns of comorbidity. Third, we advance a view of SAD as based on the
dysregulation of two specific systems, rather than on a set of symptoms. Such a
vision is consistent with recent calls to conceptualize psychological disorders
in terms of basic neuropsychological mechanisms (RDoC, [54]). Finally,
conceptualizing SAD through the lenses of the two biobehavioral systems
highlights a functionalist account of SAD, and has the potential to synthesize
diverse levels of analyses: from subjective self-repot, to cognitive,
endocrine, and neural indices. Looking at both the whole and the parts (i.e.,
systems and processes) can be
a powerful method of advancing knowledge. A system-based account with links to
specific processes can integrate the currently expansive, yet disparate,
literature on SAD.
This review highlights the organizing properties of
biobehavioral social systems as
a set of mechanisms that have a common functional theme and act as a
coordinated set of neural, hormonal, cognitive, and behavioral processes [31;
80; 81]. This overarching structure is used to synthesize and integrate a large
number of process-based findings into a more cohesive whole. The current
review also highlights the potentially impaired linkage between the two
systems [36].
We first present data on patterns of reactions of high- and low-SA individuals to challenges in the affiliation (exclusion) and social-rank (defeat) domains. We then discuss the possibility of enhanced coupling of the systems in SAD. Next, we explore the ways in which this account helps integrate clinically significant information about SAD. We conclude by reviewing established and novel theory-based directions for treatments of SAD, and sketch directions for future research.
SAD and the Affiliation System: Dealing with Exclusion
Over the course of evolution, conspecifics depended on each other, with those able to garner social support increasing their chances of survival. Such natural selection pressures led to the evolution of a biobehavioral system of affiliation, which continuously monitors for inclusionary status, and uses this information to guide behavior [12; 86]. Affiliation behavioral system emerges early in the developmental sequence, operates automatically and fluently, is attuned to specific social cues such as touch, gaze, and vocalization, and involves specific endocrine pathways [31; 37; 38; 97]. The brain circuitry dedicated to the affiliation biobehavioral system integrates sub-cortical survival-related networks with insula-cingulate and frontotempoparietal networks [26]. Social exclusion may be elicited by discrete cues (e.g., looking away) or by complex events, such as social exclusion [109].
Social exclusion has been found to affect behavioral, physiological, motivational, endocrine, and neural responses [102]. For example, excluded individuals are more sensitive to emotional tone and more accurate in distinguishing between real and posed smiles [14; 69]. Moreover, exclusion appears to be associated with an enhanced activation of the noradrenergic component of the sympathetic nervous system and with greater activity in dACC and anterior insula [26; 110]. Importantly, individuals differ in their regulation of responses to exclusion: whereas some attempt to initiate or enhance pro-social behaviors, others withdraw from interpersonal contact [93]. These regulatory differences are related to dispositional traits of the excluded individuals (targets), the relationship between excluders and target (in-group, out-group), and perceived expectancies of relationship repair and/or opportunities for reaffiliation [19; 89; 109].
Interpersonal rejection and social exclusion are common occurrences in the daily lives of high-SA individuals [32]. Yet, high-SAs are found to exhibit a more intense, less self-regulated, and more protracted response to exclusion than do low-SA individuals [36]. Whereas a common response to exclusion in non-SAs includes an enhanced attempt to reconnect with others, high-SA individuals appear to “down-regulate” the system: they exhibit a drop in progesterone [64], selectively attend to threat rather than to affiliation cues [94], and do not enhance the positive evaluation of new potential interaction partners [62]. In sum, the functioning of subjective, cognitive, and hormonal components suggest that high-SA individuals tend to withdraw from affiliative opportunities following exclusion events.
SAD and the Social-Rank System: Dealing with Defeat
Group living confers evolutionary advantages, but the primary cost of such an arrangement is the need to compete with conspecifics for important resources [7; 57; 100]. The social-rank biobehavioral system monitors the relative social standing of conspecifics, and uses this information to guide behavior [87]. It also emerges early in the developmental sequence, operates automatically and fluently, is attuned to nonverbal signals, and involves specific endocrine pathways [38]. Data from human and animal studies suggest that specific brain circuits specialize in processing social-rank related information [8]. For humans, losing a competition, being publicly criticized and failing to achieve the expected degree of social influence is experienced as social defeat [95; 111].
Social defeat is among the most stressful events for humans and animals alike [56], affecting response layers from subjective to cognitive, expressive, endocrine, and neural [24]. Specifically, loss of social-rank is associated with increases in feelings of shame, sensitivity to cues of dominance, blood pressure, cortisol levels, and inflammatory responses [56]. Defeat may elicit distinct reactions ranging from submission, subordination and ingratiation [63; 108] to expressions of dominance and aggression [112].
A robust body of literature suggests that SAD is associated with submissive response to dominance cues [33]. For example, Maner and colleagues found that high-SA, but not low-SA, men exhibited a drop in testosterone following defeat [63]. Similarly, high-SA individuals demonstrated an increase in pitch and a decrease in postural expansion when competing over female attention [103]. High-SA individuals also exhibited more signals of low-dominance during social tasks, such as rigidness, fidgeting and gaze avoidance [104]. Combined, evidence encompassing subjective, expressive, and hormonal data suggests that SA is associated with a down-regulation of social-rank system, although this relationship is stronger in men than in women.
Defeat-Exclusion linkage in SAD
In everyday life exclusion and defeat may co-occur [109]. Targets of exclusion may feel not only disliked but also demoted and disrespected [76; 107]. It appears that high-SA individuals are more inclined than low-SA individuals to interpret exclusion as significant threat to their relational value and self-esteem [41]. These interpretations may, in turn, trigger a pattern of cognitions and behaviors which are akin to social defeat [73].
When exclusion connotes defeat, low-SAs often attempt to
reinstate their social value via aggression [23], conspicuous consumption [59],
or pursuit of risky financial opportunities [25]. High-SA individuals, however,
appear to respond to exclusion by
a mixture of social withdrawal and submission, such as decrease in vocal
confidence [33; 62] and submissive interpersonal tactics [77]. In a daily diary
study, perception of low-affiliative behaviors in a communication partner were
linked to submissive behaviors more strongly in individuals diagnosed with SAD
than in non-clinical controls [79]. Combined, these data suggest that SA is
associated with a tendency to interpret exclusion as a threat to both
affiliation and social standing, and to forgo strategies geared to bolster
them. Figure graphically depicts the proposed differences in the reactions of
high- and low-SA individuals to two common social challenges of defeat and
exclusion.
Figure. A Dual-System Model of SAD
Clinical Profile of SAD from the Perspective of the Dual-System Model
Given its early onset, SAD has been called an “adolescent disorder” [43]. Adolescence is a period of neural plasticity, of enhanced emphasis on formation of social bonds and establishment of social status [22]. Accordingly, adolescents are especially vulnerable to both exclusion and defeat, and exhibit enhanced reactivity on affective, neural, and hormonal measures [40; 50; 84; 92]. The increased onset of SAD during adolescence may be attributable to the heightened functioning of both the affiliation and social-rank biobehavioral systems during this period. Indeed, prevention and intervention efforts may be especially targeted to this sensitive period.
Gender affects the severity, expression, developmental
trajectory, and interpersonal costs incurred by individuals with SAD [21].
Increase in the onset of SAD during adolescence is particularly pronounced for
girls, with early sexual maturation presenting
a particular risk factor for this gender [15]. Men are more likely to incur the
interpersonal cost of social defeat leading to less frequent opportunities to
establish intimate bonds whereas women enlist social bonds to form exclusionary
alliances [13]. Specifically, men with SAD appear to entail greater costs to
intimate relationship than do women, possibly due to their enhanced submissive
strategies in response to social setbacks [113]. Consistent with our
dual-system account, these findings paint a gender-specific pattern of
vulnerabilities for affiliation and social-rank threats in SAD.
The high comorbidity between SAD and MDD is not surprising given the sensitivity to exclusion and defeat common to both conditions. Indeed, enhanced sensitivity to social rejection is at the core of several models of depression [68; 88]. Moreover, maladaptive tendency to withdraw in response to social rejection by strangers is also common to both conditions [52; 55]. Some patterns of reaction to exclusion, however, appear to be disorder-specific: depressed individuals seek closeness and reassurance from their significant others whereas high-SA individuals appear to ward off closeness by decreasing self-disclosure [106].
SAD is also highly comorbid with substance abuse disorders, especially alcohol [16]. High-SA individuals appear to use alcohol to self-medicate, allegedly seeking to ease the anxiety in anticipation of, and during, social interactions. Recent research linked social exclusion to drinking [11; 71], and to disinhibited behavior following drinking [90]. In summary, SAD individuals’ alcohol consumption may be used not only to attenuate the cognitive and motivational aspects of anticipatory anxiety, but also the pain of social setbacks.
State-of-the-Art Treatments and Promising Treatment Targets
CBT is the most well-research treatment for SAD. A typical course of therapy begins with psychoeducation regarding the condition, and includes a rationale for the proposed intervention. Next, the majority of sessions are devoted to a combination of exposure (“behavioral experiments”) and cognitive restructuring. At the onset of this sequence of sessions, a personalized plan of exposures is constructed collaboratively by the client and the therapist. The importance of reduction of self-focused attention and decrease in safety-seeking behaviors is emphasized. Exposure in SAD constitutes a deliberate and systematic series of actions geared to approach socially threatening situations such as social gathering or job interviews. Exposure experiences have a clear set of goals in which the person’s expectations are put to test. At the end of exposure “data” collected during the experience are processed, and anxiety-related and negative self-relevant beliefs are challenged. Typically, the emphasis in exposure is to increase the patient’s presence and assertiveness in social interactions. CBT thus focuses on counteracting the down-regulation of the social-rank system. Treatment is typically terminated once the pre-determined sequence of exposures has been completed, and concludes with an assessment of the progress made.
Although CBT has shown consistent effectiveness in alleviating SAD severity, many patients still fail to respond or exhibit only partial recovery. Such failures may be due, at least in part, to a rather partial view of vulnerability in SAD, highlighting deficits associated with the functioning of the social-rank system, and focusing almost exclusively on cognitive and expressive components. According to the presented conceptualization, simultaneously targeting multiple components of response to social challenges is key to enhancing the efficacy of SAD treatments.
Some novel interventions propose to specifically target
cognitive, endocrine, and neural components of reactions to social threats and
setbacks. Cognitive bias modification programs (CBMs) seek to train individuals
to attend affiliative stimuli to or interpret ambiguous social events in a
benign manner. For example, interpretation training reduced SA-severity
possibly by training individuals to re-appraise exclusion events, and
attentional procedure were used to implicitly train individuals to re-focus on
affiliative stimuli
[58; 96]. Attentional CBT were found to attenuate anxious response to
subsequent stressor, as well as to enhance positive affect [82].
Embodiment-based interventions target the expressive component by modifying facial and bodily expressions related to the production of affiliative and dominant non-verbal signals [38]. In non-clinical populations such interventions lead to congruent changes in affect and cognitive processing. For example, adopting powerful postures and lowering one’s pitch reduced interpersonal fearfulness [53; 91].
Based on the compromised endocrine response to exclusion, translational pharmacological methods have begun to enhance existing therapeutic procedures via administration of pro-affiliatory hormones such as oxytocin [29]. In addition, based on the findings of reduced testosterone levels in SAD [39] and the fact that testosterone administration can shift social avoidance to social approach [98], suggestions are raised for the inclusion of testosterone administration as an enhancer to boost the effects of exposure therapy in SAD [28].
Excitatory brain stimulation to enhance control over automatic social avoidance has been proposed [101]. Indeed, an anodal transcranial direct stimulation over an area which is postulated to be responsible for the regulation of social pain (rVLPFC) reduced distress following exclusion [74]. The same procedure was also found to reduce the relationship between social exclusion and aggression [75]. Such stimulation techniques may prove helpful in a range of clinical conditions involving hypersensitivity to exclusion.
The need for personalized interventions has been emphasized by authors reviewing state-of-the-art treatments for SAD [49]. Patient-specific conceptualization of regulatory repertoire in the face of social threats may prove valuable to clinicians in designing focused, individually-tailored intervention. The understanding of the range and diversity of this repertoire may indicate foci where treatment effectiveness can be enhanced [66].
Future Directions
The present model provides but a first step in the creation of a comprehensive account of SAD in which system-level disturbances are linked to specific processes. Several directions for future research are apparent. First, data regarding individual differences in response to exclusion and defeat are only beginning to accumulate. Second, the causal status of responses to exclusion and defeat in the etiology and maintenance of SAD is yet unclear (see, however, [60]). Third, a more extensive effort needs to be dedicated to understand the impact of favorable social events (e.g., inclusion, social ascent) on SAD [36]. Finally, more research is needed to examine the flexibility of responses to exclusion and defeat, namely, the ability to select an appropriate, context-specific, regulatory response strategy. Clearly, such flexibility is likely to determine adaptive functioning [85].
Concluding Remarks
We argued that individuals with SAD are characterized by a heightened sensitivity to social challenges, by a propensity to respond to these challenges by down-regulating the triggering system, and by an enhanced coupling between the affiliative and the social-rank systems. Exploring the mechanisms used to maintain a stable, yet flexible balance between the need to belong and the need to matter can help understand and treat SAD.
References
-
Barkovich A.J., Norman D. Anomalies of the corpus
callosum: Correlation with further anomalies of the brain. American Journal
of Neuroradiology, 1988. Vol. 9, no. 3,
pp. 493–501. Acarturk C., Cuijpers P., VanStraten A. et al. Psychological treatment of social anxiety disorder: A meta-analysis. Psychological Medicine, 2009. Vol. 39, no. 2, pp. 241–254. DOI: 10.1017/S0033291708003590 - Aderka I.M., Hofmann S.G. (in press). Process-based Treatment for Social Anxiety. In D.H. Barlow (Ed.), Clinical Handbook of Psychological Disorders. New York, NY: The Guilford Press.
- Aderka I.M., Hofmann S.G., Nickerson A. et al. Functional impairment in social anxiety disorder. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 2012. Vol. 26, no. 3, pp. 393–400. DOI: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2012.01.003
-
Aderka I.M.,
Weisman
O., Shahar
G. et al.
The roles of the social rank and attachment systems in social anxiety.
Personality and Individual Differences, 2009. Vol. 47, no. 4,
pp. 284–288. DOI: 10.1016/j.paid.2009.03.014 - Alden L.E., Buhr K., Robichaud M. et al. Treatment of social approach processes in adults with social anxiety disorder. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 2018. Vol. 86, no. 6, pp. 505–517. DOI: 10.1037/ccp0000306
- American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 5th Edition. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Publishing, Inc., 2013. DOI: 10.1176/appi.books.9780890425596.744053
-
Anderson C., Hildreth J.A.D., Howland L. Is the desire for
status a fundamental human motive? A review of the empirical
literature. Psychological Bulletin, 2015. Vol. 141, no.
3,
pp. 574–601. DOI: 10.1037/a0038781 - Arviv O., Goldstein A., Weeting J. et al. Brain response to the M170 time interval is sensitive to socially relevant information. Neuropsychologia, 2015. Vol. 78, pp. 18–28. DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2015.09.030
- Asher M., Hermesh H., Gur S. et al. Do men and women arrive, stay, and respond differently to cognitive behavior group therapy for social anxiety disorder? Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 2019. Vol. 64, pp. 64–70. DOI: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2019.03.005
- Asher M., Asnaani A., Aderka I.M. Gender differences in social anxiety disorder: A review. Clinical Psychology Review, 2017. Vol. 56, pp. 1–12. DOI: 10.1016/j.cpr.2017.05.004
- Bacon A.K., Engerman B. Excluded, then inebriated: A preliminary investigation into the role of ostracism on alcohol consumption. Addictive Behaviors Reports, 2018. Vol. 8, pp. 25–32. DOI: 10.1016/j.abrep.2018.05.002
-
Baumeister R.F., Leary M.R. The need to belong: desire for
interpersonal attachments as a fundamental human motivation. Psychological
Bulletin, 1995. Vol. 117,
no. 3, pp. 497–529. DOI: 10.1037/0033-2909.117.3.497 - Benenson J.F., Antonellis T.J., Cotton B.J. et al. Sex differences in children’s formation of exclusionary alliances under scarce resource conditions. Animal Behaviour, 2008. Vol. 76, no. 2, pp. 497–505. DOI: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2008.01.027
-
Bernstein M.J., Young S.G., Brown C.M. et al. Adaptive
responses to social exclusion: Social rejection improves detection of real and
fake smiles. Psychological Science, 2008.
Vol. 19, no. 10, pp. 981–983. DOI: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02187.x - Blumenthal H., Leen-Feldner E.W., Babson K.A. et al. Elevated social anxiety among early maturing girls. Developmental Psychology, 2011. Vol. 47, no. 4, pp. 1133–1140. DOI: 10.1037/a0024008
- Buckner J.D., Lewis E.M., Walukevich-Dienst K. Drinking problems and social anxiety among young adults: The roles of drinking to manage negative and positive affect in social situations. Substance Use & Misuse, 2019. Vol. 54, no. 13, pp. 2117–2126. DOI: 10.1080/10826084.2019.1637892
- Bugental D.B. Acquisition of the algorithms of social life: A domain-based approach. Psychological Bulletin, 2000. Vol. 126, no. 2, pp. 187–219. DOI: 10.1037/0033-2909.126.2.187
- Clark D.M., Wells A. A cognitive model of social phobia. In R.G. Heim-Berg, M.R. Liebowitz, D.A. Hope, F.R. Schneier (Eds.), Social Phobia: Diagnosis, Assessment, and Treatment. New York, NY: Guilford Press, 1995, pp. 69–93.
-
Cooley S., Burkholder A.R., Killen M. Social inclusion and
exclusion in same-race and interracial peer encounters. Developmental
Psychology, 2019. Vol. 55, no. 11,
pp. 2440–2450. DOI: 10.1037/dev0000810 -
Craske M.G., Niles A.N., Burklund L.J. et al. Randomized
controlled trial of cognitive behavioral therapy and acceptance and commitment
therapy for social phobia: Outcomes and moderators. Journal of
Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 2014. Vol. 82, no. 6,
pp. 1034–1048. DOI: 10.1037/a0037212 -
Crome E., Baillie A. Social anxiety disorder diagnostic
criteria perform equally across age, comorbid diagnosis, and
performance/interaction subtypes. Anxiety, Stress,
& Coping, 2015. Vol. 28, no. 2, pp. 179–191. DOI: 10.1080/10615806.2014.930445 -
Crone E.A., Dahl R.E. Understanding adolescence as a
period of social–affective engagement and goal flexibility. Nature Reviews
Neuroscience, 2012. Vol. 13, no. 9,
pp. 636–650. DOI: 10.1038/nrn3313 - DeWall C.N. Forming a basis for acceptance: Excluded people form attitudes to agree with potential affiliates. Social Influence, 2010. Vol. 5, no. 4, pp. 245–260. DOI: 10.1080/15534511003783536
- Diaz V. Lin D. Neural circuits for coping with social defeat. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 2020. Vol. 60, pp. 99–107.DOI: 10.1016/j.conb.2019.11.016
- Duclos R., Wan E.W., Jiang Y. Show Me the Honey! Effects of Social Exclusion on Financial Risk-Taking. Journal of Consumer Research, 2013. Vol. 40, no. 1, pp. 122–135. DOI: 10.1086/668900
- Eisenberger N.I. Social Pain and the Brain: Controversies, Questions, and Where to Go from Here. Annual Review of Psychology, 2015. Vol. 66, pp. 601–629. DOI: 10.1146/annurev-psych-010213-115146
- Eng W., Coles M.E., Heimberg R.G., et al. Domains of life satisfaction in social anxiety disorder: Relation to symptoms and response to cognitive-behavioral therapy. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 2005. Vol. 19, no. 2, pp. 143–156. DOI: 10.1016/ j.janxdis.2004.01.007
- Enter D., Spinhoven P., Roelofs K. Alleviating social avoidance: Effects of single dose testosterone administration on approach–avoidance action. Hormones and Behavior, 2014. Vol. 65, no. 4, pp. 351–354. DOI: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2014.02.001
- Fang A., Hoge E.A., Heinrichs M. et al. Attachment Style Moderates the Effects of Oxytocin on Social Behaviors and Cognitions During Social Rejection: Applying a Research Domain Criteria Framework to Social Anxiety. Clinical Psychological Science, 2014. Vol. 2, no. 6, pp. 740–747. DOI: 10.1177/2167702614527948
- Fehm L., Beesdo K., Jacobi F. et al. Social anxiety disorder above and below the diagnostic threshold: Prevalence, comorbidity and impairment in the general population. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 2008. Vol. 43, no. 4, pp. 257–265. DOI: 10.1007/s00127-007-0299-4
- Feldman R. The neurobiology of human attachments. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 2017. Vol. 21, no. 2, pp. 80–99. DOI: 10.1016/j.tics.2016.11.007
- Festa C.C., Ginsburg G.S. Parental and Peer Predictors of Social Anxiety in Youth. Child Psychiatry & Human Development, 2011. Vol. 42, no. 3, pp. 291–306. DOI: 10.1007/s10578-011-0215-8
- Gilboa-Schechtman E., Galili L., Sahar Y. et al. Being “in” or “out” of the game: subjective and acoustic reactions to exclusion and popularity in social anxiety. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 2014. Vol. 8, article number 147. DOI: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00147
- Gilboa-Schechtman E., Keshet H., Livne T. et al. Explicit and implicit self-evaluations in social anxiety disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 2017. Vol. 126, no. 3, pp. 285–290. DOI: 10.1037/abn0000261
- Gilboa-Schechtman E., Keshet H., Peschard V. et al. Self and Identity in Social Anxiety Disorder. Journal of Personality, 2019. Vol. 88, no. 1, pp. 106–121. DOI: 10.1111/jopy.12455
- Gilboa-Schechtman E., Shachar I., Helpman L. Evolutionary Approaches to Social Anxiety. In S.G. Hofmann, P.M. DiBartolo (Eds.), Social Anxiety: Clinical, Developmental, and Social Perspectives (3rd ed.). Waltham. MA: Elsevier, 2014, pp. 599–625.
- Gilboa-Schechtman E. Shachar I., Sahar Y. Positivity impairment as a broad-based feature of social anxiety. In J. Weeks (Ed.), Handbook on Social Anxiety Disorder. New-York, NY: Wiley-Blackwell, 2014, pp. 409–433.
- Gilboa-Schechtman E., Shachar-Lavie I. More than a face: a unified theoretical perspective on nonverbal social cue processing in social anxiety. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 2013. Vol. 7, article number 904. DOI: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00904
- Giltay E.J., Enter D., Zitman F.G. et al. Salivary testosterone: Associations with depression, anxiety disorders, and antidepressant use in a large cohort study. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 2012. Vol. 72, no. 3, pp. 205–213. DOI: 10.1016/j.jpsychores. 2011.11.014
- Gunther Moor B., Güroğlu B., Op de Macks Z.A. et al. Social exclusion and punishment of excluders: Neural correlates and developmental trajectories. NeuroImage, 2012. Vol. 59, no. 1, pp. 708–717. DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.07.028
- Gutz L., Roepke S., Renneberg B. Cognitive and affective processing of social exclusion in borderline personality disorder and social anxiety disorder. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 2016. Vol. 87, pp. 70–75. DOI: 10.1016/j.brat.2016.08.020
- Haker A., Aderka I.M., Marom S. et al. Impression formation and revision in social anxiety disorder. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 2014. Vol. 28, no. 2, pp. 133–139. DOI: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2013.05.001
- Haller S.P.W., Kadosh K., Scerif F. et al. Social Anxiety disorder in adolescence: How developmental cognitive neuroscience findings may shape understanding and interventions for psychopathology. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 2015. Vol. 13, pp. 11–20. DOI: 10.1016/j.dcn.2015.02.002
- Hawley P.H. The duality of human nature: Coercion and prosociality in youths’ hierarchy ascension and social success. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 2014. Vol. 23, no. 6, pp. 433–438. DOI: 10.1177/0963721414548417
-
Heimberg R.G., Brozovich F.A., Rapee R.M. A
cognitive-behavioral model of social anxiety disorder. In S.G. Hofmann, P.M.
DiBartolo (Eds.), Social Anxiety: Clinical, Developmental, and Social
Perspectives (3rd ed.). Waltham, MA: Elsevier, 2014,
pp. 705–728. DOI: 10.1016/B978-0-12-394427-6.00025-8 - Hermans E.J., van Honk J. Toward a framework for defective emotion processing in social phobia. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, 2006. Vol. 11, no. 3, pp. 307–331. DOI: 10.1080/13546800500213993
- Herres J., Shearer A., Kodish T. et al. Differences in suicide risk severity among suicidal youth with anxiety disorders. Crisis: The Journal of Crisis Intervention and Suicide Prevention, 2019, Vol. 40, no. 5, pp. 333–339. DOI: 10.1027/0227-5910/a000571
-
Hofmann S.G. Cognitive factors that maintain social
anxiety disorder:
A comprehensive model and its treatment implications. Cognitive Behaviour Therapy, 2007. Vol. 36, no. 4, pp. 193–209. DOI: 10.1080/16506070701421313 - Hofmann S.G. Hayes S.C. The future of intervention science: Process-based therapy. Clinical Psychological Science, 2018. Vol. 7, no. 1. DOI: 10.1177/2167702618772296
-
Holder M.K., Blaustein J.D. Puberty and adolescence as a
time of vulnerability to stressors that alter neurobehavioral processes.
Frontiers in Neuroendocrinology, 2014.
Vol. 35, no. 1, pp. 89–110. DOI: 10.1016/j.yfrne.2013.10.004 - Hoyer J., Wiltink J., Hiller W. et al. Baseline patient characteristics predicting outcome and attrition in cognitive therapy for social phobia: results from a large multicentre trial. Clinical Psychology &Psychotherapy, 2014. Vol. 23, no. 1, pp. 35–46. DOI: 10.1002/cpp.1936
- Hsu D.T., Sanford B.J., Meyers K.K. et al. It still hurts: altered endogenous opioid activity in the brain during social rejection and acceptance in major depressive disorder. Molecular Psychiatry, 2015. Vol. 20, no. 2, pp. 193–200. DOI: 10.1038/mp.2014.185
- Huang L., Galinsky A.D., Gruenfeld D.H. et al. Powerful postures versus powerful roles: Which is the proximate correlate of thought and behavior? Psychological Science, 2011. Vol. 22, no. 1, pp. 95–102. DOI: 10.1177/0956797610391912
-
Insel T., Cuthbert B., Garvey M. et al. Research Domain
Criteria (RDoC): Toward
a New Classification Framework for Research on Mental Disorders. American Journal of Psychiatry, 2010. Vol. 167, no. 7, pp. 748–751. DOI: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2010.09091379 - Jobst A., Sabass L., Palagyi A. et al. Effects of social exclusion on emotions and oxytocin and cortisol levels in patients with chronic depression. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 2015. Vol. 60, pp. 170–177. DOI: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2014.11.001
-
Kemeny M.E. Psychobiological responses to social threat:
Evolution of
a psychological model in psychoneuroimmunology. Brain, Behavior, and Immunity, 2009. Vol. 23, no. 1, pp. 1–9. DOI: 10.1016/j.bbi.2008.08.008 - Kraus M.W.,Torrez B. A psychology of power that is embedded in societal structures. Current Opinion in Psychology, 2020. Vol. 33, pp. 86–90. DOI: 10.1016/ j.copsyc.2019.07.018
- Lau J.Y.F., Pettit E., Creswell C. Reducing children’s social anxiety symptoms: Exploring a novel parent-administered cognitive bias modification training intervention. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 2013. Vol. 51, no. 7, pp. 333–337. DOI: 10.1016/ j.brat.2013.03.008
- Lee J., Shrum L.J. Conspicuous consumption versus charitable behavior in response to social exclusion: A differential needs explanation. Journal of Consumer Research, 2012. Vol. 39, no. 3, pp. 530–544. DOI: 10.1086/664039
- Levinson C.A., Langer J.K., Rodebaugh T.L. Reactivity to exclusion prospectively predicts social anxiety symptoms in young adults. Behavior Therapy, 2013. Vol. 44, no. 3, pp. 470–478. DOI: 10.1016/j.beth.2013.04.007
- Locke K.D. Agentic and communal social motives. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 2015. Vol. 9, no. 10, pp. 525–538. DOI: 10.1111/spc3.12201
- Mallott M.A., Maner J.K., DeWall N. et al. Compensatory deficits following rejection: the role of social anxiety in disrupting affiliative behavior. Depression and Anxiety, 2009. Vol. 26, no. 5, pp. 438–446. DOI: 10.1002/da.20555
- Maner J.K., Miller S.L., Schmidt N.B. et al. Submitting to Defeat: Social Anxiety, Dominance Threat, and Decrements in Testosterone. Psychological Science, 2008. Vol. 19, no. 8, pp. 764–768. DOI: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02154.x
- Maner J.K., Miller S.L., Schmidt N.B., Eckel L.A. The Endocrinology of Exclusion: Rejection Elicits Motivationally Tuned Changes in Progesterone. Psychological Science, 2010. Vol. 21, no. 4, pp. 581–588. DOI: 10.1177/0956797610362676
- Mayo-Wilson E., Dias S., Mavranezouli I. et al. Psychological and pharmacological interventions for social anxiety disorder in adults: a systematic review and network meta-analysis. The Lancet Psychiatry, 2014. Vol. 1, no. 5, pp. 368–376. DOI: 10.1016/S2215-0366(14)70329-3
- Moscovitch D.A. What Is the Core Fear in Social Phobia? A New Model to Facilitate Individualized Case Conceptualization and Treatment. Cognitive and Behavioral Practice, 2009. Vol. 16, no. 2, pp. 123–134. DOI: 10.1016/j.cbpra.2008.04.002
- Ohayon M.M., Schatzberg A.F. Social phobia and depression: Prevalence and comorbidity. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 2010. Vol. 68, no. 3, pp. 235–243. DOI: 10.1016/j.jpsychores.2009.07.018
-
Pharo H., Gross J., Richardson R. et al. Age-related
changes in the effect
of ostracism. Social Influence, 2011. Vol. 6, no. 1, pp. 22–38. DOI: 10.1080/ 15534510.2010.525852 - Pickett C.L., Gardner W.L., Knowles M. Getting a cue: the need to belong and enhanced sensitivity to social cues. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 2004. Vol. 30, no. 9, pp. 1095–1107. DOI: 10.1177/0146167203262085
- Pull Ch.B. Current status of knowledge on public-speaking anxiety. Current Opinion in Psychiatry, 2012. Vol. 25, no. 1. pp. 32–28. DOI: 10.1097/YCO.0b013e32834e06d
- Rabinovitz S. Drowning your sorrows? Social exclusion and anger effects on alcohol drinking. Addiction Research & Theory, 2014. Vol. 22, no. 5, pp. 363–370. DOI: 10.3109/16066359.2013.857661
- Rapee R.M., Heimberg R.G. A cognitive-behavioral model of anxiety in social phobia. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 1997. Vol. 35, no. 8, pp. 741–756. DOI: 10.1016/S0005-7967(97)000223
- Riva P., Montali L., Wirth J.H. et al. Chronic social exclusion and evidence for the resignation stage: An empirical investigation. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 2017. Vol. 34, no. 4, pp. 541–564. DOI: 10.1177/0265407516644348
- Riva P., Romero Lauro L.J., DeWall C.N., Bushman B.J. Buffer the Pain Away: Stimulating the Right Ventrolateral Prefrontal Cortex Reduces Pain Following Social Exclusion. Psychological Science, 2012. Vol. 23, no. 12, pp. 1473–1475. DOI: 10.1177/0956797612450894
- Riva P., Romero Lauro L.J., DeWall C.N. et al. Reducing aggressive responses to social exclusion using transcranial direct current stimulation. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 2015. Vol. 10, no. 3, pp. 352–356. DOI: 10.1093/scan/nsu053
- Robertson T.E., Sznycer D., Delton A.W. et al. The true trigger of shame: Social devaluation is sufficient, wrongdoing is unnecessary. Evolution and Human Behavior, 2018. Vol. 39, no. 5, pp. 566–573. DOI: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2018.05.010
-
Romero-Canyas R., Downey G., Reddy K.S. et al. Paying to
belong: When does rejection trigger ingratiation? Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 2010. Vol. 99,
no. 5, pp. 802–823. DOI: 10.1037/a0020013 - Ruscio A.M., Brown T.A., Chiu W.T. et al. Social fears and social phobia in the USA: results from the National Comorbidity Survey Replication. Psychological Medicine, 2008. Vol. 38, no. 1, pp. 15–28. DOI: 10.1017/S0033291707001699
- Sadikaj G., Moskowitz D.S., Russell J.J. et al. Submissiveness in Social Anxiety Disorder: The Role of Interpersonal Perception and Embarrassment. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 2015. Vol. 34, no. 1, pp. 1–27. DOI: 10.1521/jscp.2015.34.1.1
- Sapolsky R.M. The influence of social hierarchy on primate health. Science (New York, N.Y.), 2005. Vol. 308(5722), pp. 648–652. DOI: 10.1126/science.1106477
- Schaller M. The behavioural immune system and the psychology of human sociality. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, 2011. Vol. 366, pp. 3418–3426, DOI: 10.1098/rstb.2011.0029
- Schnabel K., Asendorpf J.B. Cognitive trainings reduce implicit social rejection associations. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 2015. Vol. 34, no. 5, pp. 365–391. DOI: 10.1521/jscp.2015.34.3.1
- Schneier F.R., Foose T.E., Hasin D.S. et al. Social anxiety disorder and alcohol use disorder co-morbidity in the national epidemiologic Survey on alcohol and related conditions. Psychological Medicine, 2010. Vol. 40, no. 6, pp. 977–988. DOI: 10.1017/S0033291709991231
-
Sebastian C., Viding E., Williams K.D. et al. Social brain
development and the affective consequences of ostracism in adolescence.
Brain and Cognition, 2010. Vol. 72,
no. 1, pp. 134–145. DOI: 10.1016/j.bandc.2009.06.008 - Sheppes G., Suri G., Gross J.J. Emotion regulation and psychopathology. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 2015. Vol. 11, no. 1, pp. 379–405. DOI: 10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-032814-112739
-
Shilling A.A., Brown C.M. Goal-driven resource
redistribution: An adaptive response to social exclusion. Evolutionary
Behavioral Sciences, 2016. Vol. 10, no. 3,
pp. 149–167. DOI: 10.1037/ebs0000062 - Silk J.B. Social components of fitness in primate groups. Science, 2007. Vol. 317(5843), pp. 1347–1351. DOI: 10.1126/science.1140734
-
Slavich G.M., O’Donovan A., Epel E.S. et al. Black sheep
get the blues:
A psychobiological model of social rejection and depression. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 2010. Vol. 35, no. 1, pp. 39–45. DOI: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.01.003 - Smart Richman L., Leary M.R. Reactions to discrimination, stigmatization, ostracism, and other forms of interpersonal rejection: a multimotive model. Psychological Review, 2009. Vol. 116, no. 2, pp. 365–383. DOI: 10.1037/a0015250
- Sprunger J.G., Hales A., Maloney M. et al. Alcohol, affect, and aggression: An investigation of alcohol’s effects following ostracism. Psychology of Violence, 2020. Advance online publication. DOI: 10.1037/vio0000341
- Stel M., van Dijk E., Smith P.K. et al. Lowering the pitch of your voice makes you feel more powerful and think more abstractly. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 2012. Vol. 3, no. 4, pp. 497–502. DOI: 10.1177/1948550611427610
- Sumter S.R., Bokhorst C.L., Miers A.C. et al. Age and puberty differences in stress responses during a public speaking task: Do adolescents grow more sensitive to social evaluation? Psychoneuroendocrinology, 2010. Vol. 35, no. 10, pp. 1510–1516.
- Sunami N., Nadzan M.A., Jaremka L.M. Does the prospect of fulfilling belonging affect social responses to rejection? A conceptual replication attempt. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 2019. Vol. 10, no. 3, pp. 307–316. DOI: 10.1177/ 1948550618762301
- Tanaka H., Ikegami T. Fear of negative evaluation moderates effects of social exclusion on selective attention to social signs. Cognition and Emotion, 2015. Vol. 29, no. 7, pp. 1306–1313. DOI: 10.1080/02699931.2014.977848
- Taylor C.J. Physiological stress response to loss of social influence and threats to masculinity. Social Science & Medicine, 2014. Vol. 103, pp. 51–59. DOI: 10.1016/ j.socscimed.2013.07.036
- Taylor C.T., Bomyea J., Amir N. Attentional bias away from positive social information mediates the link between social anxiety and anxiety vulnerability to a social stressor. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 2010. Vol. 24, no. 4, pp. 403–408. DOI: 10.1016/ j.janxdis.2010.02.004
-
Taylor S.E., Klein L.C., Lewis B.P. et al. Biobehavioral
responses to stress in females: tend-and-befriend, not fight-or-flight.
Psychological Review, 2000. Vol. 107, no. 3,
pp. 411–429. DOI: 10.1037/0033-295x.107.3.411 - Terburg D., Aarts H., van Honk J. Testosterone Affects Gaze Aversion From Angry Faces Outside of Conscious Awareness. Psychological Science, 2012. Vol. 23, no. 5, pp. 459–463. DOI: 10.1177/0956797611433336
- Trower P., Gilbert P. New theoretical conceptions of social anxiety and social phobia. Clinical Psychology Review, 1989. Vol. 9, no. 1, pp. 19–35. DOI: 10.1016/0272-7358(89)90044-5
- van Kleef G.A. Cheng J.T. Power, status, and hierarchy: Current trends and future challenges. Current Opinion in Psychology, 2020. Vol. 33, v-xiv. DOI: 10.1016/j.copsyc. 2020.03.011
- Volman I., Toni I., Verhagen L. et al. Endogenous testosterone modulates prefrontal-amygdala connectivity during social emotional behavior. Cerebral Cortex, 2011. Vol. 21, no. 10, pp. 2282–2290. DOI: 10.1093/cercor/bhr001
-
Wang
H., Braun
Ch., Enck P.
How the brain reacts to social stress (exclusion) –
A scoping review. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 2017. Vol. 80, pp. 80–88. DOI: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.05.012 -
Weeks J.W., Heimberg R.G., Heuer R. Exploring the role of
behavioral submissiveness in social anxiety. Social and Clinical
Psychology, 2011. Vol. 30, no. 3,
pp. 217–249. DOI: 10.1521/jscp.2011.30.3.217 - Weeks J.W., Howell A.N., Srivastav A. et al. “Fear guides the eyes of the beholder”: Assessing gaze avoidance in social anxiety disorder via covert eye tracking of dynamic social stimuli. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 2019. Vol. 65, pp. 56–63. DOI: 10.1016/ j.janxdis.2019.05.005
- Weeks J.W., Rodebaugh T.L., Heimberg R.G. et al. To avoid evaluation, withdraw: Fears of evaluation and depressive cognitions lead to social anxiety and submissive withdrawal. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 2008. Vol. 33, no. 4, pp. 375–389. DOI: 10.1007/s10608-008-9203-0
- Weisman O., Aderka I.M., Marom S. et al. Social rank and affiliation in social anxiety disorder. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 2011. Vol. 49, no. 6-7, pp. 399–405. DOI: 10.1016/j.brat.2011.03.010
- Westermann S., Rief W., Euteneuer F. et al. Social exclusion and shame in obesity. Eating Behaviors, 2015. Vol. 17, pp. 74–76. DOI: 10.1016/j.eatbeh.2015.01.001
- Wiemers U.S., Schultheiss O.C., Wolf O.T. Public speaking in front of an unreceptive audience increases implicit power motivation and its endocrine arousal signature. Hormones and Behavior, 2015. Vol. 71, pp. 69–74. DOI: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2015.04.007
-
Williams K.D. Ostracism. Annual Review of
Psychology, 2007. Vol. 58, no. 1,
pp. 425–452. DOI: 10.1146/annurev.psych.58.110405.085641 - Williamson T.J., Thomas K.S., Eisenberger N.I. et al. Effects of social exclusion on cardiovascular and affective reactivity to a socially evaluative stressor. International Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 2018. Vol. 25, no. 4, pp. 410–420. DOI: 10.1007/s12529-018-9720-5
- Wirth M.M., Welsh K.M., Schultheiss O.C. Salivary cortisol changes in humans after winning or losing a dominance contest depend on implicit power motivation. Hormones and Behavior, 2006. Vol. 49, no. 3, pp. 346–352. DOI: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2005.08.013
- Wood S. Individual Differences in the Neurobiology of Social Stress: Implications for Depression-Cardiovascular Disease Comorbidity. Current Neuropharmacology, 2014. Vol. 12, no. 2, pp. 205–211. DOI: 10.2174/1570159X11666131120224413
- Xu Y., Schneier F., Heimberg R.G. et al. Gender differences in social anxiety disorder: Results from the national epidemiologic sample on alcohol and related conditions. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 2012. Vol. 26, no. 1, pp. 12–19. DOI: 10.1016/ j.janxdis.2011.08.006
Information About the Authors
Metrics
Views
Total: 969
Previous month: 16
Current month: 10
Downloads
Total: 480
Previous month: 3
Current month: 2