Psychological Restoration and Nature Connectedness in terms of "Bottom-up" and "Top-down" Approaches to Understanding Interaction with Nature

112

Abstract

Relevance. The article presents an overview of notions about the foreign environmental psychology constructs — psychological restoration and nature connectedness. The constructs represent the effects of person's relationship with nature and are thought of as a resource of health/well-being. Objective. To reveal general characteristics of the constructs and to analyze them from the point of view of the "bottom-up" (evolutionary-psychological) and "top-down" (constructivist) approaches to understanding the salutogenic effects of interaction with nature. Methods. Analysis and generalization of theoretical and empirical research within the framework of foreign and Russian environmental psychology. Results. The constructs are characterized using the following criteria: basic conceptual assumptions, specific constructs, measures, empirical support. The theoretical foundations of constructs and their role in empirical studies are considered from the point of view of "bottom-up" and "top-down" approaches. Based on the review, we identified a trend towards the integration of approaches, and highlighted points that should be taken into account when studying the salutogenic effects of interaction with nature. Among them are mediation and moderation schemes, which include variables of psychological restoration and nature connectedness, they are "predisposed" to support "bottom-up" and "top-down" assumptions, respectively. When studying restorative environments, it is worth predicting the likely influence of “top-down” variables: traits, values, attitudes. In Russian studies, mostly appears the construct of connection with nature

General Information

Keywords: psychological restoration, nature connectedness, bottom-up and top-down approaches, well-being, environmental psychology

Journal rubric: Developmental Psychology

Article type: scientific article

DOI: https://doi.org/10.17759/chp.2023190411

Received: 06.12.2023

Accepted:

For citation: Shatalova O.V. Psychological Restoration and Nature Connectedness in terms of "Bottom-up" and "Top-down" Approaches to Understanding Interaction with Nature. Kul'turno-istoricheskaya psikhologiya = Cultural-Historical Psychology, 2023. Vol. 19, no. 4, pp. 109–118. DOI: 10.17759/chp.2023190411.

Full text

Introduction

The concept of the beneficial effects of the natural environment on human mental and physical health – such as relaxation, restoration, and vitality – has increasingly become the focus of psychological studies over the past two decades [52]. This growing interest is associated, according to experts, with concerns regarding the quality of the surrounding environment amidst advancing urbanization. This is rooted in the widely accepted notion that the environment has the potential to influence human health and well-being [13; 19; 24]. In the field of environmental psychology, research into salutogenic (supporting health and well-being) effects of nature has emerged as an actively evolving area in international science (North America, Europe, Australia, East Asia) [24; 28; 36].

This article presents a narrative review of scientific perspectives on two specific constructs within this domain: psychological restoration and nature connectedness. Each construct represents a fragment of the psychological reality influenced by human interaction with nature. Interaction can be with nature as a referent (physical reality) or with nature as a sign, but one way or another it produces certain changes in the human psyche. Both constructs are popular in environmental psychology, corresponding with specific areas of research. However, given their significance in understanding the psychological benefits of interacting with nature [9; 32; 54], their spheres are increasingly overlapping (see Table 1).

Table 1. The Number of Search Results in Google Scholar

Keywords

Year of Publication

1990−1999

2000−2009

2010−2019

2020−2023

psychology

restoration

nature connectedness

2

20

643

934

Note: Search Parameters: Any articles; All of these words appear anywhere in the article: psychology, restoration, and the exact phrase nature connectedness.

The tasks of this article are: 1) to provide a general characteristic of constructs, 2) to examine the constructs and their possible relationships from the perspective of "bottom-up" and "top-down" approaches to understanding the salutogenic effects of nature, as the discussion of these approaches has become an area of significant focus in environmental psychology in recent years [20; 29; 40; 47; 51; 52]. "Bottom-up" theories are based on evolutionary psychological foundations and universalize the beneficial influence of the physical qualities of the environment on a person. "Top-down" theories explain the effects of nature not through the immanent qualities of the stimulus, but through a person's interpretation of this stimulus. The choice of the topic is determined by the scarcity of texts addressing these tasks in English and the extreme scarcity in Russian.

General Characteristics of Constructs

The term psychological restoration [32] or restoration [21], concerning the effects of being in nature, has become established due to the influence of well-known theories since the 1970s: Attention Restoration Theory (ART) by R. and S. Kaplan and Stress Recovery Theory (SRT) by R. Ulrich [22, p. 95]. The former focuses on restoring cognitive functions, particularly directed attention, after mental fatigue (cognitive restoration), while the latter focuses on reducing vegetative stress responses (physiological restoration) and improving emotional states by decreasing negative emotions and increasing positive ones (affective restoration) [43, p. 7]. Both frameworks are evolutionarily oriented and explain the effects of being in nature through the evolutionary benefits of perceiving its qualities [52, p. 38]. To some extent polemical, these theories in the 20th century demonstrated an inclination toward synthesis [27], which is now realized: contemporary empirical research often considers restoration as a complex phenomenon that combines cognitive and affective/affective-physiological recuperation [21; 30; 31; 53] (literature also includes social and other dimensions of restoration [43], yet here we adhere to the "traditional" boundaries of the concepts outlined by ART and SRT [9]). One definition of this complex phenomenon is "the recovery of physical and psychological adaptive resources depleted in the performance of daily activities" [21, p. 154].

As noted by one of the leading researchers on nature's effects, T. Hartig, the construct of restoration belongs to the framework of the relationship between an individual and their environment, which he, following S. Saegert and G. Winkel, terms "the adaptive paradigm" [22, p. 91]. Restoration is one facet of adaptation, alongside two others expressed in terms of stress and coping. The basic conceptual assumption of this construct is the need for periodic renewal of mental adaptive resources [Ibid.].

Empirical studies on the salutogenic effects of nature contact encompass various aspects of the restoration phenomenon, in other words, it's a general construct comprising several specific ones. For instance, one can distinguish between restoration and restorativeness [21]. "Restoration" emphasizes the individual's state as a result of their interaction with the environment. Its indicators may be subjective, such as when specific self-report scales are used (Restoration Outcome Scale [30]), or objective, in cases employing measurements of physiological (pulse, cortisol level) or cognitive (objective tests) restoration. "Restorativeness" emphasizes a person's evaluation of the environment. This evaluation assesses the specific restorative characteristics of the environment described by ART [27], for which specialized questionnaires are used (Perceived Restorativeness Scale [23]). One study mentioned over 10 self-report restoration/restorativeness scales [33]; no equivalent original or adapted tools have been identified in Russian psychology.

Although the heterogeneity of the listed measures leads to divergent results, the overall positions of ART and SRT are confirmed. For instance, a systematic review of 36 studies verifies the reliability of results for affective restoration, showing less consistent outcomes for physiological restoration [17]. A systematic review of 42 works confirms cognitive restoration but with a greater effect on working memory and cognitive flexibility than on attentional control [50]. A meta-analysis of 22 studies comparing restoration self-report scales confirms a more pronounced restorative potential of natural environments compared to urban ones [39].

In Russian psychology, the concept of restoration is represented in a limited manner, both in theoretical [2] and empirical research. Among the latter, one can mention organizational-psychological studies of work environments that consider natural elements and symbols integrated into office spaces as resources for psychological restoration [4; 5].

The terms used to define the second discussed general construct, nature connectedness, in foreign psychology vary: nature affiliation [25], nature connectedness [14], sense of connection, relationship with nature, environmental identity [45, p. 109], human-nature connectedness [10]. The underlying category behind these names circulates between "sense (of oneness)" [34], "relationship" [48], "identity" [45; 48]. Currently, there is no widely accepted definition; the concept is rather intuitively grasped. One definition of this construct is "sense of oneness with the natural world" [34, p. 504]. Nature connectedness can be conceptualized as a state induced by contact with nature [35; 41], or as a personal trait [35; 42], where an individual feels this connection "regardless of where one is" [9, p. 13].

Specific constructs of nature connectedness correspond to a range of psychodiagnostic instruments; empirically, their commonality is affirmed by a strong correlation of their indicators [45]. Among these concepts are emotional closeness to nature (Emotional Affinity Toward Nature scale); cognitive aspects (Implicit Associations Test – Nature); both cognitive and affective components (Connectedness to Nature Scale (hereinafter – CNS)); a combination of emotional, cognitive, and behavioral aspects (Nature Relatedness Scale); environmental identity (Environmental Identity scale (hereinafter – EID)) [Ibid.], among others. One study examines 26 tools measuring similar phenomena [49]. Russian research utilizes adapted foreign methods, such as "Шкала связи с природой" (adaption of CNS) [6], "Шкала идентификация с природой" (adaption of EID) [15], as well as original instruments: "Люди и растения" (People and Plants) [3].

The origin of the general construct is associated, on the one hand, with global urbanization [25], and on the other hand, with global environmental issues [42]. The fundamental conceptual assumption can be formulated as follows: the sense of connection with nature compensates for alienation from nature (conceptualized as a harmful deficit, "nature starvation" [25, p. 232]), and also promotes ecologically oriented attitudes and actions. In essence, this construct is conceptualized as a resource simultaneously for health/well-being and pro-environmental behavior [38]. In a systematic review of 16 meta-analyses (832 experimental and correlational studies), it was demonstrated that nature connectedness has a positive influence on pro-environmental behaviors and values, and also positively correlates with the same phenomena and with health/well-being [10]. Well-being, a positive psychological aspect of health, stands as one of the most prominent variables validating the beneficial effects of nature connectedness [45, p. 113]. A meta-analysis of 30 studies indicates that a stronger nature connectedness aligns with higher levels of hedonic well-being [14], while a similar pattern is evident concerning eudaimonic well-being in a meta-analysis of 20 studies [46]. This consistent pattern is also corroborated in Russian studies concerning the relationship between nature connectedness and well-being [1; 6; 26].

Theoretical foundations of constructs

The difference between "bottom-up" and "top-down" restoration theories lies in the explanations for the restorative effects of nature contact. "Bottom-up" theories explain these effects through evolutionary mechanisms and the "universally beneficial" properties of the environment, while "top-down" theories attribute them to sociocultural mechanisms – cognitive constructs, attitudes, and values.

Traditionally recognized as "bottom-up" theories are ART and SRT, along with the biophilia hypothesis by E. Wilson, which suggests that for successful adaptation, human ancestors needed to be emotionally responsive to non-threatening non-human life forms [9]. This need continues to contribute to subjective security and productivity in humans. "Top-down" or constructivist ideas are more recent. An example is the Conditioned Restoration Theory by L. Egner et al., which suggests that the restoration process follows the classical conditioning scheme: leisure in a natural environment triggers feelings of relaxation and pleasure, cementing the "nature+restoration" association [18]. Other "top-down" concepts, supported by empirical research, explain restoration through the influence of cognitive constructs (learned positive associations with nature) [20; 29] or personal variables (place attachment) [40; 51].

Among the explanations of nature connectedness, one can also recognize tendencies towards the discussed poles. On the "bottom-up" side, we find the same biophilia hypothesis [45, pp. 110, 112], while on the "top-down" side, there's the Self-Determination Theory by R. Ryan and E. Deci, where nature connectedness is seen as satisfaction a basic psychological need for relationships. Unlike the biophilia hypothesis, the sense of connectedness here is determined not "objectively", but subjectively (for individuals with autism, engaging with nature can fulfill the need for relationships) [16].

Particularly noteworthy is the recent trend toward integrating approaches. An example covering both discussed constructs is the work of G. Barbiero and R. Berto. On one hand, the authors conceptualize restoration, as understood by ART (1), and nature connectedness (2) as two components of biophilia, reflecting evolutionarily ingrained tendencies in humans "to focus upon life and lifelike forms" (1) and "affiliate with them emotionally" (2) [9, p. 12]. On the other hand, "top-down" explanations do not contradict the biophilia hypothesis. Biophilia is not inevitability but potential ("weak learning rules"), requiring cultivation in an individual's psyche through accumulating experience of nature contacts [8, pp. 4, 8]. Apparently, the non-realization of this potential does not deprive individuals of receiving restorative benefits. In one of the works, a hypothesis is proposed according to which the level of restoration is a product of both the nature connectedness and the "biophilic quality" (naturalness level) of the environment. A high level of restoration can be produced by a combination of high connectedness and high biophilic quality of the environment, as well as a combination of low connectedness and low biophilic quality [11, p. 14]. Thus, "bottom-up" and "top-down" logics merge at the point of implementing initial assumptions.

The other group of authors presents a similar scenario, offering an evolutionary-constructivist perspective on restoration: an inherent positive response to natural stimuli can be modified by "top-down" factors, such as the level of nature connectedness [29].

Another example of understanding restoration is interesting as a broad synthesizing gesture. This is a three-level model of restoration in urban environments, which can be adapted for natural environments as well. The first level of the model is containment: the absence of noise, pollution, crowds. There are no psychological changes at this level; it serves as a premise for restoration. The second level is passive restoration, induced by the "bottom-up features" of the urban environment, such as biophilic design. In the case of nature, these are the qualities of the environment itself. The effects of this level are the forms of restoration described by ART and SRT. The third level, active restoration, works through "top-down features", the personal contribution of the individual: place attachment, place memories, sense of belonging (in the case of a natural environment, this could be nature connectedness). The effects of the third level: cognitive engagement, eudaimonic well-being [12].

The other theoretical framework integrates restoration and nature connection "under the umbrella" of mindful engagement in nature experiences (both effects are supported by a practice of mindfulness). The authors distinguish between both perceptually-oriented external awareness and internal awareness of emotions, thoughts, and ideas. They note that in a "more restorative", i.e., more natural environment, the first form might be sufficient for psychological benefits, while in perceptually restricted conditions (plantings in a busy city) the second one might be needed [32]. It's easy to observe here the analogy between passive "bottom-up" and active "top-down" restoration.

Relationships between constructs

Given that both the constructs of restoration and nature connectedness embody positive psychological effects of engaging with the natural environment, their convergence in research domains was inevitable.

Their relationships can also be systematized based on the distinction between the bottom-up and top-down approaches. Specifically, from this standpoint, one can consider the application of mathematical-statistical methods such as mediation analysis and moderation analysis, often used to test assumptions about the relationship between the discussed constructs. The use of mediation analysis in studying the relationships between constructs is more characteristic of the "bottom-up" approach. As noted by P. Olivos and S. Clayton, nature connectedness "could be considered a mediator variable because of its possible phylogenetic origin and a potential universal explanation of the sense of belonging to nature" [45, p. 118]. The authors discuss here the mediation of relationships between variables of nature exposure and well-being. In our view, this notion can be extended to other relevant mediation schemes as mediation reconstructs the paths of effects ("how or why such effects occur" [Ibid.]), representing a universalizing assumption par excellence. Along with that, moderation analysis tests conditions ("when certain effects will hold" [Ibid.]), which constitutes a differentiating "top-down" assumption.

For instance, the mediating role of nature connectedness in the relationship between nature perception and affective-cognitive restoration has been experimentally confirmed [35]. In other words, sense of oneness with the natural world acts as the "active ingredient" in restoration. In another experiment, affective restoration mediates the effect of outdoor walks concerning nature connectedness [41]. This means that the path from nature contacts to the sense of connectedness is facilitated through affective restoration. Both studies, despite the contrasting directions of effects in regression models, pose questions and interpret results within the framework of a "bottom-up" approach.

An example validating the "top-down" hypothesis is a study confirming the moderating role of nature connectedness in the relationship between landscape perception and affective restoration [37], where higher affective restoration was observed among individuals feeling more connected to nature. Another instance is a study where nature connectedness acts as a grouping variable (that is conceptually close to moderation analysis). It was confirmed that the nature connectedness variable “switches modes” of restoration: the low-urban-oriented group (or nature-oriented) showed the highest restoration effect after a walk in the forest compared to the high-urban-oriented group [44].

In summary, depending on the theoretical framework and study design, we obtain arguments supporting both "top-down" and "bottom-up" logic of relationships. Such polyphony can be predictable for two reasons. Firstly, due to the complexity of constructs and the diversity of their indicators, not to mention the vast array of additional variables (socio-demographic characteristics, outdoor activity types, environmental types) beyond the scope of this article. Accordingly, different data configurations may cause different relationship patterns. Secondly, the approaches integration described above allows validating both positions.

Given the aforementioned, it could be suggested that the constructs are interdependent. This formulation doesn't contradict either approach, signifying that a stronger nature connectedness is observed in a more restorative natural environment, and conversely, more pronounced restoration is documented in the environment respondents feel more connected to [54]. As articulated in one recent study, restoration and nature connectedness "appear to mutually reinforce one another" [32, p. 2].

Conclusion

Psychological restoration, recovery of depleted adaptive resources through nature perception, and nature connectedness, sense of oneness with the natural world, are constructs used in environmental psychology to describe the psychological effects of human-nature relationships.

The theoretical foundations of these constructs and their relationships in empirical studies are examined through the lenses of both "bottom-up" (evolution-oriented) and "top-down" (constructivist) approaches to understanding the salutogenic effects of nature. A tendency towards integration observed in the discussed research area is highlighted. On one hand, this is integration of the approaches where the psychological effects of relationships with nature depend on both the qualities of the environment and the characteristics of a person. On the other hand, this is conglomerate of psychological effects: restoration and nature connectedness tend to be considered mutually reinforcing phenomena. Hence, in exploring the psychological benefits of nature, both constructs are increasingly taken into account in recent years.

The review's limitations include the generalizing perspective used to consider the constructs, which might create an impression of their homogeneity. However, restoration indicators are quite heterogeneous; the construct in some cases might be represented by cortisol levels in saliva [17, p. 11], while in others by evaluating statements like "I would like to spend more time looking at the surroundings" [23, p. 182]. Although there's currently no basis to isolate any parts from the basic construct, generalizing such diverse phenomena should be approached with caution. And, of course, "bottom-up" / "top-down" frameworks cannot exhaust the topic of interaction with nature.

This article might be of interest to researchers studying recovery from mental fatigue or stress and those exploring the effects of nature contacts within Russian-speaking samples. The review allows for the identification of aspects worth considering in such research. Firstly, mediation and moderation models involving restoration and nature connectedness variables are inclined to support "bottom-up" and "top-down" assumptions, respectively. Secondly, when studying restorative environments, it's important to forecast the likely influence of "top-down" variables – attitudes towards nature, value preferences, etc.

References

  1. Mukhortova E.A. Svyaz' psikhologicheskogo blagopoluchiya s otnosheniem podrostkov k prirode [Connection of psychological well-being with the attitude of teenagers to nature]. Ekopsikhologicheskiye issledovaniya—6: ekologiya detstva i psikhologiya ustojchivogo razvitiya: Sbornik nauchnykh statej [Ecopsychological research-6: ecology of childhood and psychology of sustainable development]. Moscow: Psychological Institute of RAO; Kursk: University Book, 2020, pp. 241—245 (In Russ.).
  2. Nartova-Bochaver S.K., Mukhortova Е.А., Irkhin B.D. Vzaimodeistvie s mirom rastenii kak istochnik pozitivnogo funktsionirovaniya cheloveka [Interaction with the Plant World as a Source of Positive Human Functioning]. Konsul'tativnaya psikhologiya i psikhoterapiya = Counseling Psychology and Psychotherapy, 2020. Vol. 28 (2), pp. 151—169. DOI:10.17759/cpp.2020280209 (In Russ.).
  3. Nartova-Bochaver S.K., Mukhortova Е.А. Oprosnik «Lyudi i Rasteniya» (LiR): izucheniye otnosheniya cheloveka k miru rastenij [Questionnaire “People and Plants” (PaP): a study of human relations to the plant world]. Psikhologicheskij zhurnal [Psychological journal], 2020. Vol. 41 (1), pp. 86—96. DOI:10.31857/S020595920007984-8 (In Russ.).
  4. Pavlova M.V. Biofil'nyi i salyutogennyi dizain v sozdanii «zdorovogo» ofisa [Biophilic and salutogenic design in creating a "healthy" office]. Media. Dizain [Communications. Media. Design], 2022. Vol. 7 (2), pp. 97—124. (In Russ.).
  5. Pavlova M.V., Nartova-Bochaver S.K. Rutinnye (obydennye) tekhniki samopomoshchi v ofise: na primere masterskoi arkhitektorov [Routine self-help behaviors of employees (in case of Architect offices)]. Organizatsionnaya psikhologiya [Organizational Psychology], 2020. Vol. 10 (3), pp. 164—184. (In Russ.).
  6. Chistopol’skaya K.A., Enikolopov S.N., Nikolaev E.L., Semikin G.I. Svyaz’ s prirodoj: vklad v dushevnoye blagopoluchiye [Nature connectedness and its contribution to mental well-being]. In Perspektivy psikhologicheskoj nauki i praktiki: Sbornik statej Mezhdunarodnoj nauchno-prakticheskoj konferenczii [Prospects of Psychological Science and Practice: Proceedings of International Scientific-Practical Conference]. Moscow: Kosygin Russian State University, 2017, pp. 764—767 (In Russ.).
  7. Shatalova O.V. Vosstanovitel’nyj effekt kontaktov s prirodoj kak predmet issledovanija v psihologii sredy [Restorative effect of nature contact as a subject of environmental psychology]. Zhurnal Vysshei shkoly ekonomiki [Psychology. Journal of the Higher School of Economics], 2022. Vol. 19 (4), pp. 855—871.DOI:10.17323/1813-8918-2022-4-855-871 (In Russ.).
  8. Barbiero G., Berto R. Biophilia as evolutionary adaptation: An onto- and phylogenetic framework for biophilic design. Frontiers in Psychology, 2021. Vol. 12. Article 700709. DOI:10.3389/fpsyg.2021.700709
  9. Barbiero G., Berto R. From biophilia to naturalist intelligence passing through perceived restorativeness and connection to nature. Annals of Reviews and Research, 2018. Vol. 3 (1), pp. 12—17. DOI:10.19080/ARR.2018.03.555604
  10. Barragan-Jason G., Loreau M., de Mazancourt C., Singer C.M., Parmesan C. Psychological and physical connections with nature improve both human well-being and nature conservation: A systematic review of meta-analyses. Biological Conservation. 2023. Vol. 277. Article DOI:10.1016/j.biocon.2022.109842
  11. Berto R., Barbiero G., Barbiero P., Senes G. An individual’s connection to nature can affect perceived restorativeness of natural environments. Some observations about biophilia. Behavioral Sciences. 2018. Vol. 8 (3). 34. DOI:10.3390/bs8030034
  12. Bornioli A., Subiza-Pérez M. Restorative urban environments for healthy cities: A theoretical model for the study of restorative experiences in urban built settings. Landscape Research. 2023. Vol. 48(1). P. 152—163. DOI:10.1080/01426397.2022.2124962
  13. Browning M.H.E.M., Shipley , McAnirlin O., Becker D., Yu C.-P., Hartig T., Dzhambov A.M. An actual natural setting improves mood better than its virtual counterpart: a meta-analysis of experimental data. Frontiers in Psychology. 2020. Vol. 11. Article 2200. DOI:10.3389/fpsyg.2020.02200
  14. Capaldi C.A., Dopko L., Zelenski J.M. The relationship between nature connectedness and happiness: a meta-analysis. Frontiers in Psychology. 2014. Vol. 5. Article 976. DOI:10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00976
  15. Clayton S., Irkhin B.D., Nartova-Bochaver S.K. Environmental identity in Russia: validation and relationship to the concern for people and plants. Journal of the Higher School of Economics, 2019. Vol. 16 (1), pp. 85—107. DOI:10.17323/1813-8918-2019-1-85-107
  16. Cleary A., Fielding K.S., Bell S. L., Murray Z., Roiko A. Exploring potential mechanisms involved in the relationship between eudaimonic wellbeing and nature connection. Landscape and Urban Planning, 2017. Vol. 158, pp. 119-128. DOI:10.1016/j.landurbplan.2016.10.003
  17. Corazon S.S., Sidenius U., Poulsen D.V., Gramkow M., Stigsdotter U.K. Psycho-physiological stress recovery in outdoor nature-based interventions: a systematic review of the past eight years of research. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 2019. Vol. 16 (10). Article 1711. DOI:10.3390/ijerph16101711
  18. Egner L.E., Sütterlin S., Calogiuri G. Proposing a framework for the restorative effects of nature through conditioning: conditioned restoration theory. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 2020. Vol. 17. Article DOI:10.3390/ijerph17186792
  19. Gallis C.Th. Forests for public health: a global innovative prospect for the humanity. In C.Th. Gallis, W.S. Shin (eds.), Forests for Public Health. Cambridge Scholars Publishing, 2020, pp. ix—x.
  20. Haga A., Halin N., Holmgren M., Sörqvist P. (2016). Psychological restoration can depend on stimulus-source attribution: a challenge for the evolutionary account? Frontiers in Psychology. 2016. Vol. 7. Article DOI:10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01831
  21. Han K.-Ts. Validity of self-reported Well-being Measures and Restoration Scale for emotions, attention, and physiology. Journal of Leisure Research, 2020. Vol. 52 (2), pp. 154—179. DOI:10.1080/00222216.2020.1752124
  22. Hartig T. Restoration in nature: beyond the conventional narrative. In A.R. Schutte, J.C. Torquati, J.R. Stevens (eds.), Nature and Psychology. Springer Nature Switzerland, 2021, pp. 89—151. DOI:10.1007/978-3-030-69020-5_5
  23. Hartig T., Korpela K., Evans G.W., Gärling T. A measure of restorative quality in environment. Scandinavian Housing & Planning Research, 1997. Vol. 14, pp. 175—194.
  24. Houlden V., Weich S., Porto de Albuquerque J., Jarvis S., Rees K. The relationship between greenspace and the mental wellbeing of adults: a systematic review. PLoS ONE, 2018. Vol. 13 (9). DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0203000
  25. Howell A.J., Passmore H.-A. The nature of happiness: nature affiliation and mental well-being. In C.L.M. Keyes, C. (eds.), Mental Well-Being: International Contributions to the Study of Positive Mental Health. Springer, Dordrecht, 2013, pp. 231—257. DOI:10.1007/978-94-007-5195-8_11
  26. Irkhin B.D. Who benefits from environmental identity? Studying Environmental Identity and Mental Wellbeing in Russia. Psychology in Russia: State of the Art, 2020. Vol. 13 (3), pp. 66—78. DOI:10.11621/pir.2020.0305
  27. Kaplan S. The restorative benefits of nature: toward an integrative framework. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 1995. Vol. 15, pp. 169—182.
  28. Keniger L.E., Gaston K.J., Irvine K.N., Fuller R.A. What are the benefits of interacting with nature? International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 2013. Vol. 10, pp. 913— DOI:10.3390/ijerph10030913
  29. Koivisto M., Jalava E., Kuusisto L., Railo H., Grassini S. Top-down processing and nature connectedness predict psychological and physiological effects of nature. Environment and Behavior, 2022. Vol. 54 (5), pp. 917— DOI:10.1177/00139165221107535
  30. Korpela K.M., Ylén, Tyrväinen L., Silvennoinen H. Determinants of restorative experiences in everyday favorite places. Health & Place, 2008. Vol. 14, pp. 636—652. DOI:10.1016/j.healthplace.2007.10.008
  31. Macaulay R., Johnson K., Lee K., Williams K. Comparing the effect of mindful and other engagement interventions in nature on attention restoration, nature connection, and mood. Journal of Environmental Psychology. Vol. 81. Article 101813. DOI:10.1016/j.jenvp.2022.101813
  32. Macaulay R., Lee K., Johnson K., Williams K. Mindful engagement, psychological restoration, and connection with nature in constrained nature experiences. Landscape and Urban Planning, 2022. Vol. 217. Article DOI:10.1016/j.landurbplan.2021.104263
  33. Malekinezhad F., bin Lamit H. Restoration experience measurement methods in contact with green open spaces. Preprints. 2018. Article DOI:10.20944/preprints201801.0064.v1
  34. Mayer F.S., Frantz C.M. The connectedness to nature scale: A measure of individuals’ feeling in community with nature. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 2004. Vol. 24 (4), pp. 503—515. DOI:10.1016/j.jenvp.2004.10.001
  35. Mayer F.S., Frantz C.M., Bruehlman-Senecal E., Dolliver K. Why is nature beneficial? The role of connectedness to nature. Environment and Behavior. 2009. Vol. 41 (5), pp. 607—643. DOI:10.1177/0013916508319745
  36. McMahan E.A., Estes, D. The effect of contact with natural environments on positive and negative affect: a meta-analysis. The Journal of Positive Psychology, 2015. Vol. 10 (6), pp. 507—519. DOI:10.1080/17439760.2014.994224
  37. McMahan E., Estes D., Murfin J.S., Bryan C.M. Nature connectedness moderates the effect of nature exposure on explicit and implicit measures of emotion. Journal of Positive Psychology and Wellbeing, 2018. (Retrieved from https://digitalcommons.wou.edu/fac_pubs/45) (review date: 09.01.2023).
  38. Mellor C., Botchway S., Barnes N. Gandy S. Seeding hope: restoring nature to restore ourselves. Nature restoration as an essential mental health intervention.. International Review of Psychiatry, 2022. Vol. 34 (5), pp. 541—545. DOI:10.1080/09540261.2022.2092391
  39. Menardo E., Brondino M., Hall R., Pasini M. Restorativeness in natural and urban environments: a meta-analysis. Psychological Reports, 2019. Vol. 124 (2), pp. 417—437. DOI:10.1177/0033294119884063
  40. Menatti L., Subiza-Pérez M., Villalpando-Flores A., Vozmediano L., San Juan C. Place attachment and identification as predictors of expected landscape restorativeness. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 2019. Vol. 63, pp. 36—43. DOI:10.1016/j.jenvp.2019.03.005
  41. Nisbet E.K., Zelenski J.M. Underestimating nearby nature: affective forecasting errors obscure the happy path to sustainability. Psychological Science, 2011. Vol. 22 (9), pp. 1101—1106. DOI:10.1177/0956797611418527
  42. Nisbet E.K., Zelenski J.M., Murphy A. The Nature Relatedness Scale: Linking individuals’ connection with nature to environmental concern and behavior. Environment and Behavior, 2009. Vol. 41 (5), pp. 715—740. DOI:10.1177/0013916508318748
  43. Nukarinen T., Rantala J., Korpela K., Browning M.H.E.M., Istance H.O., Surakka V., Raisamo R. Measures and modalities in restorative virtual natural environments: an integrative narrative review. Computers in Human Behavior, 2022. Vol. 126. Article DOI:10.1016/j.chb.2021.107008
  44. Ojala A., Korpela K., Tyrväinen L., Tiittanen P., Lanki T. Restorative effects of urban green environments and the role of urban-nature orientedness and noise sensitivity: A field experiment. Health & Place, 2019. Vol. 55, pp. 59-70. DOI:10.1016/j.healthplace.2018.11.004
  45. Olivos P., Clayton S. Self, nature and well-being: sense of connectedness and environmental identity for quality of life. In G. Fleury-Bahi, E. Pol, O. Navarro (eds.), Handbook of environmental psychology and quality of life research. Springer International Publishing Switzerland, 2017, pp. 107—126. DOI:10.1007/978-3-319-31416-7_6
  46. Pritchard A., Richardson M., Shefeld D., McEwan K. The relationship between nature connectedness and eudaimonic well‑being: A meta‑ Journal of Happiness Studies, 2020. Vol. 21, pp. 1145—1167. DOI:10.1007/s10902-019-00118-6
  47. Ratcliffe E., Subiza-Pérez M., Korpela K.M. Nature as support to mental health: memories of favourite natural places and their links to perceived psychological wellbeing. In C.Th. Gallis, W.S. Shin (eds.), Forests for Public Health. Cambridge Scholars Publishing, 2020, pp. 56—78.
  48. Restall B., Conrad E. A literature review of connectedness to nature and its potential for environmental management. Journal of Environmental Management, 2015. Vol. 159, pp. 264—278. DOI:10.1016/j.jenvman.2015.05.022
  49. Salazar G., Monroe M.C., Jordan C., Ardoin N.M., Beery T.H. Improving assessments of connection to nature: a participatory approach. Frontiers in Ecology and Evolution, 2021. Vol. 8. Article DOI:10.3389/fevo.2020.609104
  50. Stevenson M.P., Schilhab T., Bentsen P. Attention Restoration Theory II: a systematic review to clarify attention processes affected by exposure to natural environments. Journal of Toxicology and Environmental Health, Part B., 2018. Vol. 21 (4), pp. 227—268. DOI:10.1080/10937404.2018.1505571
  51. Subiza-Pérez M., Pasanen T., Ratcliffe E., Lee K., Bornioli A., de Bloom J., Korpela K. Exploring psychological restoration in favorite indoor and outdoor urban places using a top-down perspective. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 2021. Vol. 78. Article DOI:10.1016/j.jenvp.2021.101706
  52. vаn den Berg A.E. The natural-built distinction in environmental preference and restoration: bottom-up and top-down explanations. In A.R. Schutte, J.C. Torquati, J.R. Stevens (eds.), Nature and Psychology. Springer Nature Switzerland, 2021, pp. 31—60. DOI:10.1007/978-3-030-69020-5_3
  53. Whitburn J., Linklater W.L., Milfon N.L. Exposure to urban nature and tree planting are related to pro-environmental behavior via connection to nature, the use of nature for psychological restoration, and environmental attitudes. Environment and Behavior, 2019. Vol. 51 (7), pp. 787—810. DOI:10.1177/0013916517751009
  54. Wyles K.J., White M.P., Hattam C., Pahl S., King H., Austen M. Are some natural environments more psychologically beneficial than others? The importance of type and quality on connectedness to nature and psychological restoration. Environment and Behavior, 2019. Vol. 51 (2), pp. 111—143. DOI:10.1177/0013916517738312

Information About the Authors

Oksana V. Shatalova, master student, Faculty of Psychology, Moscow Institute of Psychoanalysis, Moscow, Russia, ORCID: https://orcid.org/0000-0001-8141-1992, e-mail: shatalova_@mail.ru

Metrics

Views

Total: 542
Previous month: 49
Current month: 33

Downloads

Total: 112
Previous month: 13
Current month: 3